Skip to main content

Interrogating (In)congruence: The Incongruent Perspective

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Identifying and Interpreting Incongruent Film Music

Part of the book series: Palgrave Studies in Audio-Visual Culture ((PSAVC))

  • 357 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter initially overviews the potential perceptual effects of various types of audiovisual (in)congruence. It highlights the challenges of measuring (in)congruence and the monodimensionality, opposition, and value often implied when binary labels are used to describe film music. A (re)definition of incongruence as a lack of shared properties in the audiovisual relationship is proposed. The incongruent perspective, a psycho-semiotic approach to studying difference in the film–music relationship, is subsequently outlined. Alongside psychological research, the perspective incorporates: poststructuralist ideas that encourage deconstructing binaries and considering how difference can defer and supplement perceived meaning; theories that recognise the active role of audiovisual difference in constructing filmic meaning; and semiotic ideas that acknowledge how film music might signify, and the role of individual perceivers and practitioners in meaning construction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 64.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    To retain focus, the following paragraphs are not a holistic summary of empirical research into the psychological impact of film music. Instead, they focus on studies that explicitly consider incongruence. For recent overviews of research in this area, please see Cohen (2014, 2016) and Tan (2017a, 2017b).

  2. 2.

    For an overview of the model’s evolution see Cohen (2013).

  3. 3.

    This model has since been presented in revised format by Lipscomb (2013).

  4. 4.

    The label subjective congruence was previously used by Kim and Iwamiya (2008) to refer to such judgements.

  5. 5.

    In out-of-phase conditions audio and visual components maintain an isochronous temporal relationship that is offset from the primary beat whereas in dissonant conditions the subdivisions do not resolve easily to multiples of the primary beat (Lipscomb , 2013, p. 194).

  6. 6.

    See Donnelly (2014) and Lipscomb (2013) for further literature reviews on this topic.

  7. 7.

    For more on the challenges of self-report measures and emotional response to music see Zentner and Eerola (2010).

  8. 8.

    Cook does not suggest that because multimedia is predicated on difference that all multimedia combinations are in someway incongruent or likely to be subjectively perceived as misfitting or inappropriate. Instead his model of multimedia outlines various ways in which different types of difference might actively contribute towards perceived meaning. The model is centred around an initial test of similarity or difference in the multimedia relationship and is discussed in greater detail later in this chapter.

  9. 9.

    For Audissino , this is a result of separatist conceptions that treat film and music individually rather than as a textual whole and because of the use of communications models that emphasise interpretation rather than formal analysis.

  10. 10.

    The synthesis within Hegelian dialectics contrasts Derrida’s (1972/2002) writings, which distinguish différance from Hegelian difference ‘at the point at which Hegel … determines difference as contradiction only in order to resolve it, to interiorize it, to lift it up … into the self-presence of an onto-theological or onto-teleological synthesis. … Différance … must sign the point at which one breaks with the system of the Aufhebung and with speculative dialectics’ (p. 44).

  11. 11.

    Interestingly, Dickinson’s analysis of the video nasties also draws on Chion’s (1994) notion of anempathy , which refers to film sound and music that appears to continue indifferently and with little regard for the concurrent narrative content or images. Anempathy is often associated with incongruence and this relationship will be explored further in subsequent chapters.

  12. 12.

    As Donnelly (2014, p. 204) highlights, Eisenstein cites the work of Koffka to illustrate his argument just a few pages after these frequently cited quotes from The Film Sense.

References

  • Audissino, E. (2017). Film/music analysis: A film studies approach. Cham: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Barthes, R. (1977). Image music text (S. Heath, Trans.). London: Fontana Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barthes, R. (1981). Theory of the text. In R. Young (Ed.), Untying the text: A post-structural reader (pp. 31–47). Boston, MA: Routledge (Original work published in 1973).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bezdek, M. A., Wenzel, W. G., & Schumacher, E. H. (2017). The effect of visual and musical suspense on brain activation and memory during naturalistic viewing. Biological Psychology, 129, 73–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolivar, V. J., Cohen, A. J., & Fentress, J. C. (1994). Semantic and formal congruency in music and motion pictures: Effects on the interpretation of visual action. Psychomusicology, 13(1), 28–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boltz, M. G. (2001). Musical soundtracks as a schematic influence on the cognitive processing of filmed events. Music Perception, 18(4), 427–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boltz, M. G. (2004). The cognitive processing of film and musical soundtracks. Memory and Cognition, 32(7), 1194–1205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boltz, M., Schulkind, M., & Kantra, S. (1991). Effects of background music on the remembering of filmed events. Memory and Cognition, 19(6), 593–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bullerjahn, C., & Güldenring, M. (1994). An empirical investigation of effects of film music using qualitative content analysis. Psychomusicology, 13(1), 99–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chion, M. (1994). Audio-vision: Sound on screen (C. Gorbman, Ed. & Trans.). New York, NY: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. J. (2013). Congruence-association model of music and multimedia: Origin and evolution. In S.-L. Tan, A. J. Cohen, S. D. Lipscomb, & R. A. Kendall (Eds.), The psychology of music in multimedia (pp. 17–47). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. J. (2014). Film music from the perspective of cognitive science. In D. Neumeyer (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of film music studies (pp. 96–130). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. J. (2016). Music in performance arts: Film, theater and dance. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music psychology (2nd ed., pp. 725–744). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. J., & Siau, Y.-M. (2008). The narrative role of music in multimedia presentations: The congruence-association model (CAM) of music and multimedia. In K. Miyazaki, Y. Hiraga, M. Adachi, Y. Nakajima, & M. Tsuzaki (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (ICMPC10) Sapporo, Japan (pp. 77–82) [DVD]. Adelaide, Australia: Causal Productions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, N. (1998). Analysing musical multimedia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, D. (2005). Bernard Herrmann’s The Ghost and Mrs. Muir: A film score guide. Lanham, MD: The Scarecrow Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costabile, K. A., & Terman, A. W. (2013). Effects of film music on psychological transportation and narrative persuasion. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(3), 316–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davison, A. (2004). Hollywood theory, non-hollywood practice: Cinema soundtracks in the 1980s and 1990s. Aldershot: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J. (1997). Of grammatology (Corrected ed., G. Chakravorty Spivak, Trans.). Baltimore, MD: The John Hopkins University Press (Original work published in 1967).

    Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J. (2002). Positions (Rev. ed., A. Bass, Trans.). London: Continuum (Original work published in 1972).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, K. (2008). Off key: When film and music won’t work together. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Donnelly, K. J. (2014). Occult aesthetics: Synchronization in sound film. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, D. (2003). Charms that soothe: Classical music and the narrative film. New York, NY: Fordham University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenstein, S. (1977). Film form: Essays in film theory (J. Leyda, Ed. & Trans.). San Diego, CA: Harcourt (Original work published in 1949).

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenstein, S. (1986). The film sense (J. Leyda, Ed. & Trans.). London: Faber and Faber (Original work published in 1943).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. J., & Simons, R. F. (2005). The impact of music on subjective and physiological indices of emotion while viewing films. Psychomusicology, 19(1), 15–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorbman, C. (1987). Unheard melodies: Narrative film music. London: BFI Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haugh, S. (2001). A historical review of the development of the concept of congruence in person-centred theory. In G. Wyatt (Ed.), Rogers’ therapeutic conditions: Evolution, theory and practice—Congruence (Vol. I, pp. 1–17). Ross-on-Wye: PCCS Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal of Psychology, 57(2), 243–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hepburn, A. (1999). Derrida and psychology: Deconstruction and its ab/uses in critical and discursive psychologies. Theory and Psychology, 9(5), 639–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoeckner, B., Wyatt, E. W., Decety, J., & Nusbaum, H. (2011). Film music influences how viewers relate to movie characters. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 5(2), 146–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ireland, D. (2012). “It’s a sin […] Using Ludwig van like that. He did no harm to anyone, Beethoven just wrote music”: The role of the incongruent soundtrack in the representation of the cinematic criminal. In C. Gregoriou (Ed.), Constructing crime: Discourse and cultural representations of crime and deviance (pp. 97–111). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ireland, D. (2015). Deconstructing incongruence: A psycho-semiotic approach toward difference in the film-music relationship. Music and the Moving Image, 8(2), 48–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ireland, D. (2017). Great expectations? The changing role of audiovisual incongruence in contemporary multimedia. Music and the Moving Image, 10(3), 21–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ireland, D., & Windsor, W. L. (2016). Exploring incongruence: Shared semantic properties and judgments of appropriateness in film-music pairings. In Proceedings of the 14th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (p. 125). San Francisco, CA: ICMPC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iwamiya, S. (2013). Perceived congruence between auditory and visual elements in multimedia. In S.-L. Tan, A. J. Cohen, S. D. Lipscomb, & R. A. Kendall (Eds.), The psychology of music in multimedia (pp. 141–164). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kassabian, A. (2001). Hearing film: Tracking identifications in contemporary hollywood film music. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K., & Iwamiya, S. (2008). Formal congruency between telop patterns and sound effects. Music Perception, 25(5), 429–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, L. (1995). Classical music and postmodern knowledge. Berkley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kulezic-Wilson, D. (2015). The musicality of narrative film. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lipscomb, S. D. (2013). Cross-modal alignment of accent structures in multimedia. In S.-L. Tan, A. J. Cohen, S. D. Lipscomb, & R. A. Kendall (Eds.), The psychology of music in multimedia (pp. 192–213). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lipscomb, S. D., & Kendall, R. A. (1994). Perceptual judgement of the relationship between musical and visual components in film. Psychomusicology, 13(1), 60–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marks, L. E. (2004). Cross-modal interactions in speeded classification. In G. Calvert, C. Spence, & B. E. Stein (Eds.), The handbook of multisensory processes (pp. 85–105). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, S. K., & Cohen, A. J. (1988). Effects of musical soundtracks on attitudes toward animated geometric figures. Music Perception, 6(1), 95–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mera, M., & Stumpf, S. (2014). Eye-tracking film music. Music and the Moving Image, 7(3), 3–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nattiez, J. J. (1990). Music and discourse: Toward a semiology of music (C. Abbate, Trans.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press (Original work published in 1987).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. (Ed.). (2003). The Penguin dictionary of mathematics (3rd ed.). London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlović, I., & Marković, S. (2011). The effect of music background on the emotional appraisal of film sequences. Psihologija, 44(1), 71–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romney, J., & Wootton, A. (1995). Introduction. In J. Romney & A. Wootton (Eds.), Celluloid jukebox: Popular music and the movies since the 50s (pp. 2–7). London: British Film Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheinberg, E. (2000). Irony, satire, parody and the grotesque in the music of Shostakovich: A theory of musical incongruities. Aldershot: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sim, G. (2013). The other person in the bathroom: Mixed emotions about cognitivist film music theory. Quarterly Review of Film and Video, 30(4), 309–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tagg, P. (2001). Kojak—50 seconds of television music: Toward the analysis of affect in popular music. New York, NY: The Mass Media Music Scholars’ Press (Original work published in 1979).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tagg, P., & Clarida, B. (2003). Ten little tunes: Towards a musicology of the mass media. New York, NY: The Mass Media Music Scholars’ Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan, S.-L. (2017a). From intuition to evidence: The experimental psychology of film music. In M. Mera, R. Sadoff, & B. Winters (Eds.), The Routledge companion to screen music and sound (pp. 517–530). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan, S.-L. (2017b). Scene and heard: The role of music in shaping interpretations of film. In R. Ashley & R. Timmers (Eds.), The Routledge companion to music cognition (pp. 363–376). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan, S.-L., Cohen, A. J., Lipscomb, S. D., & Kendall, R. A. (2013). Future research directions for music and sound in multimedia. In S.-L. Tan, A. J. Cohen, S. D. Lipscomb, & R. A. Kendall (Eds.), The psychology of music in multimedia (pp. 391–405). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, S.-L., Spackman, M. P., & Bezdek, M. A. (2007). Viewers’ interpretations of film characters’ emotions: Effects of presenting film music before or after a character is shown. Music Perception, 25(2), 135–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, S.-L., Spackman, M. P., & Wakefield, E. M. (2017). The effects of diegetic and nondiegetic music on viewers’ interpretations of a film scene. Music Perception, 34(5), 605–623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitouch, O. (2001). When your ear sets the stage: Musical context effects in film perception. Psychology of Music, 29(1), 70–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallengren, A. K., & Strukelj, A. (2015). Film music and visual attention: A pilot experiment using eye-tracking. Music and the Moving Image, 8(2), 69–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallengren, A. K., & Strukelj, A. (2018). Into the film with music: Measuring eyeblinks to explore the role of film music in emotional arousal and narrative transportation. In T. Dwyer, C. Perkins, S. Redmond, & J. Sita (Eds.), Seeing into screens: Eye tracking and the moving image (pp. 65–84). New York, NY: Bloomsbury Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyatt, G. (2001). Introduction to volume I: Congruence. In G. Wyatt (Ed.), Rogers’ therapeutic conditions: Evolution, theory and practice—Congruence, volume I (pp. viii–ix). Ross-on-Wye: PCCS Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentner, M., & Eerola, T. (2010). Self-report measures and models. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (pp. 187–222). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Ireland .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Ireland, D. (2018). Interrogating (In)congruence: The Incongruent Perspective. In: Identifying and Interpreting Incongruent Film Music. Palgrave Studies in Audio-Visual Culture. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-00506-1_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics