Skip to main content

The Cultural Individuation of Human Language Capacity and the Morphogenesis of Basic Argument-Schemata

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Morphogenesis and Individuation

Part of the book series: Lecture Notes in Morphogenesis ((LECTMORPH))

  • 730 Accesses

Abstract

Language capacity unites all existent human populations. Therefore its morphogenetic field must have originated with the species Homo sapiens (ca. 200,000 y BP). We assume that at the origin this field was only poorly expressed (via some protolanguage), but unfolded with the rise of human cultures (technologies, art, myth and other symbolic media). This development occurred in the last 100,000 y. In this context, Thom’s criticism of evolutionary theory in Darwinian terms can be reconsidered. He accepts the Darwinian principles but adds the role of morphogenesis and individuation. This article sketches the morphogenesis of basic sentence (argument) structures applying results of morphodynamics and the individuation of linguistic tools in phonemics, lexicon and grammar.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Published first in the Revista di Biologia, 76 (1) and then again in Thom (1990: 599–605) and named: “Darwin cent ans après”.

  2. 2.

    The last surviving non-human species was the late Neanderthal man in Europe (extinct between 37,000 and 30,000 y. BP), i.e. before the maximum of the last ice age (25,000–20,000 y. BP).

  3. 3.

    Thus, Neanderthal individuals seem to have mixed with individuals of the early Homo sapiens when they met around 100,000 y. BP, but mixture in Europe after 40,000 when Homo sapiens migrated massively into Europe was rare and did not prevent the extinction of the Neanderthals (possibly their offspring had health problems and rarely survived).

  4. 4.

    In a study on genetically identical mice Freund et al. (2013) showed that individual differences increase with age due in “cumulative roaming entropy” and “nonshared environment” (ibidem: 756). These behavioral differences have a neural correlate in the hippocampus and are thus of a biological nature. The question is if and how such differences may be genetically transmitted; they may have an impact on survival and reproduction.

  5. 5.

    Cf. The contributions in Wildgen and Plümacher (2009; in German), and Wildgen and Brandt (2010: English and French) to this topic.For the term "affordance" see Gibson (1966).

  6. 6.

    Currently a comparable although different language capacity is assumed for Homo neanderthalensis. It allowed for a restricted communication with Homo sapiens populations if they met in a cooperative context. Current genetic analyses showed traces of Neanderthal genes in the populations, which had left Africa, probably introduced around 100,000 y. BP into the genome of Homo sapiens.

  7. 7.

    Cf. Wildgen (2004) for a set of principles governing this process. For the sketch of an "evolutionary grammar" see Wildgen (2009).

  8. 8.

    We start with rather complex schemata. Naturally a schema of frontier and the transition of frontier lines preceded the grasp schema, see for a short summary of catastrophe theoretical semantics Wildgen (2005). It may already be observed in the behaviour of chimpanzees who control their territory and attack other chimpanzees near the frontier line in order to prevent their intruding. The semiotic relevance of this schema was discussed (in relation to evolution) in an article by Per Aage Brandt: The Principle of the WallOr How Space (may have) Shaped Semiosis. A Note (download from: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2304745 ).

  9. 9.

    In Bickerton (1984) a language bioprogram is proposed in the spirit of Chomsky’s hypothesis of some inborn grammar (“universal grammar”). We reject such mechanistic devices and assume that the biologically fixed capacities are rather cognitive and behavioural than linguistic (in a narrow sense implied by the term grammar). Therefore, a topologico-dynamic basis is more plausible than low dimensional (linear) algebraic models (generative grammars) discussed by Chomsky.

  10. 10.

    Translation by the author: “The elaboration of means in view of the realization of some goal depends (in technological and biological contexts) on a “cultural” situation existing before, and on a certain amount of stability of the external conditions.”

  11. 11.

    The “dictator” in ancient Rome was only accredited his power for a restricted time in situations of extreme endangerment. In the movie Leviathan directed by Andrey Zvyagintsev and presented at the Cannes festival in 2014, Vladimir Putin is indirectly associated with the figure of Leviathan.

References

  • Bickerton, D.: The language bioprogram hypothesis and second language acquisition. In: Rutherford, W.E. (ed.) Language Universals and Second Language Acquisition, pp. 141–161. Benjamins, Amsterdam (1984)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D.: How protolanguage became language. In: Knight, C., Michael S.-K., James, R.H. (eds.) The Evolutionary Emergence of Language. Social Functions and the Origins of Linguistic Form, pp. 264–284. Cambridge U.P., Cambridge (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  • Calvin, W.H., Bickerton, D.: Lingua es Machina. Reconciling Darwin and Chomsky with the Human Brain. MIT-Press, Cambridge (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R.: Human Evolutionary Psychology. Palgrave-Mac Millan, Princeton (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Eigen, M., Schuster, P.: The Hypercycle. A Principle of Natural Self-Organization. Springer, Berlin (1979)

    Google Scholar 

  • Freund, J., Brandmaier, A., Lewejohann, L. et al.: Emergence of individuality in genetically identical mice. Science 340(6133), 756–759 (2013)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J.J.: The Senses as Perceptual Systems. Houghten Mifflin, Boston (1966)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gil, D: Early human language was isolating-monocategorical-associational, cf (2006). http://www3.isrl.uiuc.edu/~junwang4/langev/localcopy/pdf/gil06evolang.pdf

  • Haken, H.: Principles of Brain Functioning. A Synergetic Approach to Brain Activity, Behavior and Cognition. Springer, Berlin (1996)

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauss, M.: Essai sur le don: Forme et Raison de L’échange dans les Sociétés Archaïques in Sociologie et Anthropologie. PUF, Paris (1968)

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitot-Cocorda, J.: Les catastrophes de la parole. De R. Jakobson a René Thom, Maloine, Paris (1985)

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitot, J.: Cognitive Morphodynamics. Dynamical Morphological Models of Constituency in Perception and Syntax. Lang, Bern, New York (2011)

    Google Scholar 

  • Tembrock, G.: Ökosemiose. In: Posner, R., Robering, K., Sebeok, T.A. (eds.) A Handbook on the Sign-Theoretic Foundations of Nature and Culture, pp. 571–591. de Gruyter, Berlin (2004)

    Google Scholar 

  • Tesnière, L.: Éléments de Syntaxe Structurale. Klincksieck, Paris (1959)

    Google Scholar 

  • Thom, R.: Stabilité Structurelle et Morphogenèse. Interéditions, Paris (1972)

    Google Scholar 

  • Thom, R.: Mathematical Models of Morphogenesis. Horwood (Wiley), New York (1983)

    Google Scholar 

  • Thom, R.: Apologie du Logos. Hachette, Paris (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W.: Catastrophe Theoretical Semantics. An Elaboration and Application of René Thom’s Theory. Benjamins, Amsterdam (1982)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W.: Archetypensemantik. Grundlagen für eine dynamische Semantik auf der Basis der Katastrophentheorie. Narr, Tübingen (1985)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W.: Konfiguration und Perspektive im elementaren Satz: ein dynamischer Modellansatz. In: Bahner W. (ed.) Proceedings of the Fourteenth International Congress of Linguists, Berlin, 1987, pp. 1122–1125. Akademie Verlag, Berlin (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W.: Process, Image, and Meaning. A Realistic Model of the Meanings of Sentences and Narrative Texts, Series: Pragmatics and Beyond, New Series, No. 31. Benjamins, Amsterdam (1994)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W.: Selbstorganisationsprozesse in der Phonologie. In: Wagner, K., Wolfgang W. (Hg.) Studien zur Phonologie, Grammatik, Sprachphilosophie und Semiotik, Nr. 6, pp. 123–137. BLIcK, Universitätsbuchhandlung, Reihe, Bremen

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W.: Hand und Auge. Eine Studie zur Repräsentation und Selbstrepräsentation (kognitive und semantische Aspekte). Publications of the Center: Philosophical Foundations of Science, vol. 21, Univerversity Publications, Bremen. [Hand and Eye. A Study of Representation and Self-Representation (cognitive and semantic aspects). http://www.fb10.uni-bremen.de/homepages/wildgen/pdf/handundauge.pdf

  • Wildgen, Wolfgang: Dynamical models of predication. Sprachtypologie und Universalienforschung 55(4), 403–420 (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W.: The Evolution of Human Languages. Scenarios, Principles, and Cultural Dynamics. Benjamins, Amsterdam (2004)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W.: Catastrophe theoretical models in semantics. In: Köhler, R., Altmann, G., Pietrowski, R.G. (eds.) Quantitative Linguistics/Quantitative Linguistik. An International Handbook, pp. 410–423. de Gruyter, Berlin (2005)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, Wolfgang: Sketch of an evolutionary grammar based on comparative biolinguistics. In: Röska-Hardy, L.S., Neumann-Held, E.M. (eds.) Learning from Animals? Examining the Nature of Human Uniqueness, pp. 45–59. Psychology Press, Hove (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W.: Die Sprachwissenschaft des 20. Jh.s: Versuch einer Bilanz. de Gruyter, Berlin (2010)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, W., Plümacher, M. (eds.): Prägnanter Inhalt—Prägnante Form. Thematic Volume of Zeitschrift für Semiotik, 31 (1–2). Tübingen: Stauffenburg (2010)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildgen, Wolfgang, Brandt, Per Aage (eds.): Semiosis and Catastrophes. René Thom’s Semiotic Heritage. Lang, Bern (2010)

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wolfgang Wildgen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wildgen, W. (2015). The Cultural Individuation of Human Language Capacity and the Morphogenesis of Basic Argument-Schemata. In: Sarti, A., Montanari, F., Galofaro, F. (eds) Morphogenesis and Individuation. Lecture Notes in Morphogenesis. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-05101-7_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics