Skip to main content

Kognitiv-affektive Neurowissenschaft: Emotionale Modulation des Erinnerns, Entscheidens und Handelns

  • Chapter
Klinische Psychologie & Psychotherapie

Part of the book series: Springer-Lehrbuch ((SLB))

Zusammenfassung

In den letzten zwei Jahrzehnten hat die Erforschung der Informationsverarbeitungsmechanismen und neurobiologischen Systeme, die kognitiven, emotionalen und verhaltenssteuernden Prozessen zugrunde liegen, rasante Forschritte gemacht. Dies ist nicht zuletzt der Kombination von traditionellen experimental- und allgemeinpsychologischen Methoden mit elektrophysiologischen Verfahren (z. B. Messung ereigniskorrelierter Hirnpotenziale) und funktionellen Bildgebungstechniken (z. B. funktionelle Magnetresonanztomographie, fMRT, und Positronenemissionstomographie, PET) zu danken, mit denen metabolische Korrelate der neuronalen Aktivität wie die regionale Hirndurchblutung oder der Sauerstoffverbrauch in bestimmten Hirnregionen gemessen werden können. Während der Fokus des daraus entstandenen interdisziplinären Forschungsfelds der kognitiven Neurowissenschaften (Gazzaniga, 2009) zunächst primär auf grundlegenden kognitiven Leistungen wie Wahrnehmung, Gedächtnis oder Sprache lag, werden in den letzten Jahren zunehmend auch emotionale Prozesse und deren neuronalen Grundlagen erforscht (s. z. B. Davidson, 2000; LeDoux, 1996; Rolls, 2005; Roth, 2001).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Literatur

  • Abercrombie, H.C., Kalin, N.H., Thurow, M.E., Rosenkranz, M.A. & Davidson, R.J. (2004). Cortisol variation in humans affects memory for emotionally laden and neutral information. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 505–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A.R. (1998). The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature, 393, 470–474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A.K. & Phelps, E.A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411, 305–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arnsten, A.F.T. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 410– 422.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Beauregard, M., Lévesque, J. & Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, RC165, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A. & Damasio, H. (2002). Decision-making and addiction (part I): Impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1675–1689.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A.R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293–1295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, A.R., Damasio, H. & Anderson, S.W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A.R. (2000). Emotion, decision-making, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295– 307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A, Tranel, D & Damasio, H (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123, 2189–2202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland C. & Damasio, A.R. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269, 1115–1118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A.T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, E.S. & Rinck, M. (2000). Aufmerksamkeit und Gedächtnis bei Angst und Depression. Psychologische Rundschau, 51, 67–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K.C. & Robinson, T.E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Research Reviews, 28, 309–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K.C. & Robinson, T.E. (2003). Parsing reward. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 507–513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bischof, N. (1989). Emotionale Verwirrungen oder: Von den Schwierigkeiten im Umgang mit der Biologie. Psychologische Rundschau, 46, 77–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, P.H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bohannon, J.N. (1988). Flashbulb memories for the space shuttle disaster: A tale of two stories. Cognition, 29, 179–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolte, A. & Goschke, T. (2010). Thinking and emotion: Affective modulation of cognitive processing modes. In B. Glatzeder, V. Goel, & A. von Müller (Eds.), On thinking, Vol. II: Towards a theory of thinking(S. 261–277). Parmenides Book Series. Springer Verlag: Heidelberg.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bolte, A., Goschke, T. & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition: Effects of positive and negative mood on intuitive judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological Science, 14, 416–421.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M.M., Braver, T.S., Carter, C.S., Barch, D.M. & Cohen, J.C. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G.H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129– 148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bremner, J.D., Randall, P.R., Scott, T.M., Bronen, R.A., Seibyl, J.P., Southwick, S.M. et al. (1995). MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 973–981.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 73–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, T.W. & Lovallo, W.R. (2001). Enhanced memory for emotional material following stress-level cortisol treatment in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 26, 307–317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bühringer, G., Wittchen, H.-U., Gottlebe, K., Kufeld, C. & Goschke, T. (2008). Onset and cessation of substance use disorders: Why do people change? International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17(1 Suppl.), S4–S15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cahill, L., Babinsky, R., Markowitsch, H.J. & McGaugh, J.L. (1995). The amygdala and emotional memory. Nature, 377, 295–296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cahill, L., Haier, R.J., Fallon, J., Alkire, M.T., Tang, C., Keator, D., Wu, J. & McGaugh, J.L. (1996). Amygdala activity at encoding correlated with long-term, free recall of emotional information. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 93, 8016–8021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, W.B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christianson, S.A. (1989). Flashbulb memories: Special, but not so special. Memory and Cognition, 17, 435–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christianson, S.A. & Loftus, E.F. (1991). Remembering emotional events: The fate of detailed information. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 81–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D.M., Teasdale, J.D., Broadbent, D.E., & Martin, M. (1983). Effect of mood on lexical decisions. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 175–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M.A. (Hrsg.) (1997). Recovered memories and false memories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W.A, Johnson, M.C., Raye, C.L. Gatenby, Gore, J.C. & Banaji, M.R. (2004). Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15, 806–813.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Grosset Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R.J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathology, resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. American Psychologist, 55, 1196– 1214.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R.J. & Irwin, W. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 11–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Quervain, D.J., Roozendaal, B., Nitsch, R.M., McGaugh, J.L & Hock, C. (2000). Acute cortisone administration impairs retrieval of longterm declarative memory in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 313– 314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dörner, D. (1999). Bauplan für eine Seele. Reinbek: Rowohlt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreisbach, G. (2006). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: The costs and benefits of reduced maintenance capability. Brain & Cognition, 60, 11–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreisbach, G. & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 343–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dudai, Y. (2004). The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 51–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Easterbrook, J.A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 187–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eichenbaum, H. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1982). Emotion in the human face. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Levenson, R.W. & Friesen, W.V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes between emotions. Science, 221, 1208– 1210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eslinger, P.J. & Damasio, A.R. (1985). Severe disturbances of higher cognition after bilateral frontal ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology 35, 1731–1741.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M.W. (1997). Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fast, K. & Markowitsch, H.J. (2004). Neuropsychologie des posttraumatischen Stressyndroms. In S. Lautenbacher & S. Gauggel (Hrsg.), Neuropsychologie psychischer Störungen(S. 223–248). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K. (1988). Emotional mood, cognitive style, and behavior regulation. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition and social behavior(pp. 100–120). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forman, S.D., Dougherty, G.G., Casey, B.J., Siegle, G.J., Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M. et al. (2004). Opiate addicts lack error-dependent activation of rostral anterior cingulate. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 531–537.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (2008). Emotion science. Cognitive and neuroscientific approaches to understanding human emotions. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankland, P.W. & Bontempi, B. (2005). The organization of recent and remote memories. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 119–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fujiwara, E. & Markowitsch, H.-J. (2003). Das mnestische Blockadesyndrom – hirnphysiologische Korrelate von Angst und Stress. In G. Schiepek, (Hrsg.), Neurobiologie der Psychotherapie(S. 186–212). Stuttgart: Schattauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garavan, H. & Stout, J.C. (2005). Neurocognitive insights into substance abuse. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 195–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M.S. (2009). The cognitive neurosciences(4th ed.). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M.S., Ivry, R.B. & Mangun, G.R. (2009). Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind(3rd ed.). New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbertson, M.W., Shenton, M.E., Ciszewski, A., Kasai, K., Lasko, N.B., Orr, S.P. & Pitman, R.K. (2002). Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 1242–1247.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, S.G. & Bower, G.H. (1984). Cognitive consequences of emotional arousal. In C.E. Izard, J. Kagan & R. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotion, cognition and behavior(pp. 547–588). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (1996a). Gedächtnis und Emotion: Affektive Bedingungen des Einprägens, Behaltens und Vergessens. In D. Albert & K.-H. Stapf (Hrsg.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Serie II, Band 4 Gedächtnis(S. 605–694). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (1996b). Wille und Kognition. Zur funktionalen Architektur der intentionalen Handlungssteuerung. In J. Kuhl & H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Serie IV, Band 4 Motivation, Volition und Handeln(S. 583–663). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (1996c). Lernen und Gedächtnis: Mentale Prozesse und Gehirnstrukturen. In G. Roth & W. Prinz (Hrsg.), Kopf-Arbeit: Gehirnfunktionen und kognitive Leistungen(S. 359–410). Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (1997). Implicit learning and unconscious knowledge: Mental representation, computational mechanisms, and neural structures. In K. Lamberts & D. Shanks (Eds.), Knowledge, concept and categories(pp. 247–333). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2000). Involuntary persistence and intentional reconfiguration in task-set switching. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes(pp. 331–356). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2003). Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. In S. Maasen, W. Prinz & G. Roth (Eds.), Voluntary action: Brains, minds, and sociality(pp. 49–85). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2004). Vom freien Willen zur Selbstdetermination. Kognitive und volitionale Mechanismen der intentionalen Handlungssteuerung. Psychologische Rundschau, 55, 186–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2006a). Exekutive Funktionen: Kognitive Kontrolle intentionaler Handlungen. In K. Pawlik (Hrsg.), Handbuch der Psychologie(S. 249-261). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2006b). Gedächtnis: Kognitive Prozesse, emotionale Modulation und neuronale Systeme. In B. Strauß, F. Hohagen & F. Caspar (Hrsg.), Lehrbuch der Verhaltenstherapie(S. 93–110). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2008). Volition und kognitive Kontrolle. In J. Müsseler (Hrsg.). Allgemeine Psychologie (2. Aufl., S. 230–293). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grawe, K. (2004). Neuropsychotherapie. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J.J. (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J.J. (2008). Emotion regulation. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, and L.F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions(3rd edn., S. 497–512). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, O. & Goschke, T. (2004). Executive control emerging from dynamic interactions between brain systems mediating language, working memory and attentional processes. Acta Psychologica, 115, 105–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gurvits, T.V., Shenton, M.E., Hokama, H., Ohta, H., Lasko, N.B., Gilbertson, M.W. et al. (1996). Magnetic resonance imaging study of hippocampal volume in chronic, combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 1091–1099.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 394–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hardt, O., Einarsson, E.O., Nader, K. (2010). Bridge over troubled water: Reconsolidation as a link between cognitive and neuroscientific memory research traditions. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 141– 167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hariri, A.R. & Forbes, E. (2009). Genetic modulation of corticolimbic circuitry and emotion regulation. In T.E. Goldberg & D.R. Weinberger (Eds.), Genetics of Cognitive Neuroscience(S. 145–158). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hester, R. & Garavan, H. (2004) Executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction: Evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate and cerebellar activity. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 11017–11022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A.M. (1999). Positive affect. In T. Dalgleish & M.J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion(pp. 521–539). Chichester: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A.M., Johnson, M.M.S., Mertz, E. & Robinson, G.F. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1413–1426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.J. & Nadel, L. (1985). Stress-induced recovery of fears and phobias. Psychological Review, 92, 512–531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1894). The physical basis of emotion. Psychological Review, 1, 516–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalat, J.W. & Shiota, M. (2007). Emotion. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J.N., Ross, T.J., Stein, E A. & Garavan, H. (2003). Cingulate hypoactivity in cocaine users during a GO/NOGO task as revealed by event-related fMRI. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 7839–7843.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J.F. & Schacter, D.L. (2000). Functional amnesia. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology(2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 409–427). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J.J. & Diamond, D.M. (2002). The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity, and lost memories. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 453– 462.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J.J. & Fanselow, M.S. (1992). Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science, 256, 675–677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschbaum, C., Wolf, O.T., May, M., Wippich, W. & Hellhammer, D.H. (1996). Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Scienes, 58, 1475–1483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klüver, H. & Bucy, P.C. (1937). »Psychic blindness« and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Physiology, 119, 352–353

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1983a). Emotion, Kognition und Motivation: I. Auf dem Wege zu einer systemtheoretischen Betrachtung der Emotionsgenese. Sprache & Kognition, 2, 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1983b). Emotion, Kognition und Motivation: II. Die funktionale Bedeutung der Emotionen für das problemlösende Denken und für das konkrete Handeln. Sprache & Kognition, 4, 228–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional mediators of cognitive-behavior consistency: Self-regulatory processes and actions versus state orientation. In: J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action control: From cognition to behavior. (S. 101–128). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (2001). Motivation und Persönlichkeit: Interaktionen psychischer Systeme. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. & Goschke, T. (1994). State orientation and the activation and retrieval of intentions from memory. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation(S. 127–154). Göttingen/Toronto: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. & Helle, P. (1986). Motivational and volitional determinants of depression: The degenerated-intention hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 247–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhlmann, S., Kirschbaum, C. & Wolf, O.T. (2005). Effects of oral cortisol treatment in healthy young women on memory retrieval of negative and neutral words. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 83, 158– 162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lange, C.G. (1885). Om Sindsbevoegelser: Et psykofysiologiske Studie. Kopenhagen: Kronar (deutsch 1887: Über Gemuethsbewegungen. Leipzig: Theodor Thomas).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lautenbacher, S. & Gauggel, S. (2004). Neuropsychologie psychischer Störungen. Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R.S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R.S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39, 124–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R.S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 23, 155–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (2003). The synaptic self. How our brains become who we are. Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lhermitte, F. (1983). Utilization behavior’ and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106, 237–255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E.F. (2003). Our changeable memories: Legal and practical implications. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 231–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E.F. & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed memory: False memories and allegations of sexual abuse. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E.F., Loftus, G.R. & Messo, J. (1987). Some facts about »Weapon Focus«. Law and Human Behavior, 11, 55–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubman, D.I., Yücel, M. & Pantelis, C. (2004). Addiction, a condition of compulsive behaviour? Neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence of inhibitory dysregulation. Addiction, 99, 1491–1502.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, P.D. (1949). Psychosomatic disease and the ›visceral brain‹: recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosomatic Medicine, 11, 338–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. & Tata, P. (1986). Perceptual bias with emotional stimuli in normal and abnormal populations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maia, T.V. & McClelland, J.L. (2004) A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: What participants really know in the Iowa gambling Task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U. S. A., 101, 16075–16080

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markowitsch, H.J. (1999). Gedächtnisstörungen. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markowitsch, H.J. (2001). Mnestische Blockaden als Stress- und Traumfolgen. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 30, 204–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markowitsch, H.J., Calabrese, P., Wurker, M., Durwen, H.F., Kessler, J., Babinsky, R. et al. (1994). The amygdala’s contribution to memory – A study on two patients with Urbach-Wiethe disease. Neuroreport, 5, 1349–1352.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, G.D. & Zimbardo, P.G. (1979). Affective consequences of inadequately explained physiological arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 970–988.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., Naughton, B.L. & O’Reilly, R.C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419–457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S.M., York, M.K. & Montague, P.R. (2004). The neural substrates of reward processing in humans: the modern role of fMRI, Neuroscientist, 10, 260–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McEwen (2000). Effects of adverse experiences for brain structure and function. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 721–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGaugh, J.L. (2000) Memory: A century of consolidation. Science287, 248–251

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGaugh, J.L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 1–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNally, R.J. (2003). Progress and controversy in the study of posttraumatic stress disorder. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 229–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J. & Jacobs, W.J. (1998). Emotional memory. The effects of stress on »cool« and »hot« memory systems. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 38, 187–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, W.-U., Reisenzein, R. & Schützwohl, A. (2001). Einführung in die Emotionspsychologie, Band 1: Die Emotionstheorien von Watson, James und Schacter. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, W.-U., Schützwohl, A. & Reisenzein, R. (1999). Einführung in die Emotionspsychologie, Band 2 Evolutionspsychologische Emotionstheorien. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E.K. & Cohen, J.D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S. & Nugent, K. (1995). Mood-congruent memory biases in anxiety and depression. In D. Schacter (Ed.), Memory distortion. How minds, brains, and societies reconstruct the past(pp. 173–193). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R.L.C. & Phillips, L.H. (2007). The psychological, neurochemical and functional neuroanatomical mediators of the effects of positive and negative mood on executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 45, 617–629

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Kentish, J. & Bradley, B.P. (1993). Effects of anxiety and awareness on colour-identification latencies for emotional words. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 559–567.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, J.S., Öhman, A. & Dolan, R.J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393, 467–470.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, J.S., Öhman, A. & Dolan, R.J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating »unseen« fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 96, 1680–1685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, G.E. & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentelle Beiträge zur Lehre vom Gedächtnis. Zeitschrift für Psychologie (Ergänzungsband), 1, 1–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nader, K. (2003). Memory traces unbound. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 65–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1982). Snapshots or benchmarks? In U. Neisser (Ed.), Memory observed. Remembering in natural contexts(pp.43–48). San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochsner, K.N., Bunge, S.A., Gross, J.J. & Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2002). Rethinking Feelings: An fMRI Study of the Cognitive Regulation of Emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1215–1229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ofshe, R.J. & Watters, E. (1994). Making monsters: False memories, psychotherapy, and sexual hysteria. New York: Charles Schribner’s Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olds, B. & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 419–427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 148–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L. & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a ›low road‹ to ›many roads‹ of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(11), 773–783.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E.A., O’Connor, K.J., Cunningham, W.A., Funayama, E.S., Gatenby, J.C., Gore, J.C. & Banaji, M.R. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729–738.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion. A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack, R.A. & Packard, M.G. (2003). Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia 1497, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polivy, J. (1998). The effects of behavioral inhibition: integrating internal cues, cognition, behavior, and affect. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 181– 204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pope, K. (1996). Memory, abuse, and science: Questioning claims about the false memory syndrome epidemic. American Psychologist, 51, 957–974.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pope, H.G. Jr, Hudson, J.I., Bodkin, J.A. & Oliva, P. (1998). Questionable validity of »dissociative amnesia« in trauma victims. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 210–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reimann, E.M. (1997). The application of positron emission tomography to the study of normal and pathological emotion. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 58, 4–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisenzein, R. (1983). The Schachter theory of emotion: Two decades later. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 239–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reisenzein, R. &, Meyer, W.U. & Schützwohl, A. (2003). Einführung in die Emotionspsychologie: Band 3 Kognitive Emotionstheorien. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, T.E. & Berridge, K.C. (2003). Addiction. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 25–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E.T. (2005). Emotion explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E.T., Hornak, J., Wade, D. & McGrath, J. (1994). Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 1518–1524.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, G. (2001). Fühlen, Denken, Handeln. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, G., Hirsh, J.B., & Anderson, A.K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 104(1), 383–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S. & Singer, J.E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D.L. (1987). Implicit memory: history and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. (1996). Searching for memory. The brain, the mind, and the past. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. (2001). The seven sins of memory: how the mind forgets and remembers. New York: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D.L., Wagner, A.D. & Buckner R.L. (2000). Memory systems of 1999. In E.Tulving & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory(pp. 627–643). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schardt, D., Erk, S. Nüsser, C., Nöthen, M.M., Cichon, S., Rietschel, M. et al., (2010). Volition diminishes genetically mediated amygdala hyperreactivity. Neuroimage, 53(3), 943–951

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. (1990). Theorien und aktuelle Probleme der Emotionspsychologie. In K.R. Scherer (Hrsg.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Teilband C/IV/3, Psychologie der Emotion(S. 1–38). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K., Schorr, A. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.). (2001). Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiepek, G. (2003). Neurobiologie der Psychotherapie. Stuttgart: Schattauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J.W. & Eich, E. (2000). Memory for emotional events. In E. Tulving & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory(pp. 379– 392). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. (2000). Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 199–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sokolowski, K. (2002). Emotion. In J. Müsseler & W. Prinz (Hrsg.), Allgemeine Psychologie(S. 337–384). Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speisman, J.C., Lazarus, R.S., Mordkoff, A.M. & Davison, L.A. (1964). Experimental reduction of stress based on ego-defence theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 68, 367–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L.R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. (1994). Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. In D.L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems 1994(pp. 203–232). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M. & Frensch, P. (Eds.). (1998). Handbook of implicit learning. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, D. (2003). Cognitive-affective neuroscience of depression and anxiety disorders. London: Martin Dunitz Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D.T. & Knight, R.T. (2002). Frontal lobe function. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tomb, I., Hauser, M., Deldin, P. & Caramazza, A. (2002). Do somatic markers mediate decisions on the gambling task? Nature Neuroscience, 5, 1103–1104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. & Craik, F.I.M. (Eds.). (2000). The Oxford handbook of memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullsperger, M. & von Cramon, D.Y. (2003). Funktionen frontaler Strukturen. In H.-O. Karnath & P. Thier (Hrsg.), Neuropsychologie(S. 505– 514). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Kolk, B.A. (1997). The psychobiology of PTSD. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 16–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkow, N.D. & Li, T.-K. (2004). Drug addiction: The neurobiology of behaviour gone awry. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 93–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, H., von Kalckreuth, A., Schardt, D., Stephan, A., Goschke, T., & Erk, S. (2009). The temporal dynamics of voluntary emotion regulation. PLoS ONE, 4(8): e6726.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S.H. & Morris, R.G.M. (2010). Hippocampal-neocortical Interactions in memory formation, consolidation, and reconsolidation. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 49–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. (2010). The student’s guide to cognitive neuroscience. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, P.J., Rauch, S.L., Etcoff, N.L., McInerney, S.C., Lee, M.B. & Jenike, M.A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 411–418.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J.M.G., Watts, F.N., MacLeod, C. & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders(2nd ed.). Chichester: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Berridge, K.C. & Wilbarger, J.L. (2005). Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 121–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, O.T. (2003). HPA axis and memory. Best Practice and Research Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 17, 287–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1905). Grundriß der Psychologie(7. Aufl.). Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyvell, C.L. & Berridge, K.C. (2000). Intra-accumbens amphetamine increases the conditioned incentive salience of sucrose reward: Enhancement of reward »wanting« without enhanced »liking« or response reinforcement. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 8122–8130.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Goschke, T., Dreisbach, G. (2011). Kognitiv-affektive Neurowissenschaft: Emotionale Modulation des Erinnerns, Entscheidens und Handelns. In: Wittchen, HU., Hoyer, J. (eds) Klinische Psychologie & Psychotherapie. Springer-Lehrbuch. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-13018-2_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics