Skip to main content

Abstract

Until recently, there has been relatively little interest in relationships between attention and action. When modern research into attention began in the 1950s, the dominating approach was to analyze attention within the information-processing framework, and information processing was conceptualized mainly in terms of what happens to stimulus information after it has entered the human processing system (e. g., Broadbent, 1958, 1971; Neisser, 1967). Attentional processes were viewed as modifying the flow of information by selecting part of the incoming information for processing. Most of the theoretical discussions since the 1960s have centered around the question of where in the processing sequence this selection takes place. This has usually been phrased as the problem of whether selective attention operates “early,” i. e., prior to complete stimulus analysis, or “late,” after the information in the stimulus has been fully identified (e. g. Deutsch Deutsch, 1963; Treisman Geffen, 1967). Common to both theoretical positions has been the conviction that not all incoming information can be dealt with simultaneously by the system because processing capacity is limited. Hence, selection was thought to be necessary. Attention was thus conceptualized as related to the analysis and internal representation of incoming information rather than to the control of action.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aldersen, G.J.K., Sully, D.J., Sully, H.G. (1974). An operational analysis of one handed catching using high-speed photography. Journal of Motor Behavior, 6, 217–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D.A. (1977). On knowing the meaning of words we are unable to report: The effects of visual masking. In S. Domic (Ed.), Attention and performance 6 (pp. 505–533 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D.A. (1980). Attention and performance. In G. Claxton (Ed.), Cognitive psychology New directions (pp. 112–153 ). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D.A. (1987). Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action. In H. Heuer A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 395–419 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D.A. (1988). What concept of consciousness?. In A.J. Marcel E. Bisiach (Eds.), Consciousness in contemporary science (pp. 159–182 ). Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D.A., Tipper, S.P., Chmiel, N.R.J. (1985). Perceptual integration and postcategorical filtering. In M.I. Posner O.S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance 11 (pp. 107–132 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altman, J. (1978). Three levels of mentation and the hierarchic organization of the human brain. In G.A. Miller E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Psychology and biology of language and thought (pp. 87–109 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antes, J.R., Penland, J.G., Metzger, R.L. (1981). Processing global information in briefly presented pictures. Psychological Research, 43, 277–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M.A. (1981). Perceptual structures and distributed motor control. In V.Brooks (Ed.), Handbook of physiology: Sec. 1. The nervous system: Vol. 2. Motor Control. Part 2 (pp. 1449–1480 ). Be– thesda, MA: American Physiological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M.A., House, D.H. (1987). Depth and detours: An essay on visually guided behavior. In M.A. Arbib A.R. Hanson (Eds.), Vision, brain, and cooperative computation (pp. 129–163 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Averbach, E., Coriell, A.S. (1961). Short–term memory in vision. Bell Systems Technical Journal, 40, 309–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ballard, H. (1986). Cortical connections and parallel processing: Structure and function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 67–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. (1983). Automatic semantic activation and episodic memory encoding. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 88–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, H.B. (1986). Why have multiple cortical areas? Vision Research, 26, 81–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, A.S., Taylor K.W. (1979). The interaction of stimulus information with potential stimulus significance in eliciting the skin conductance orienting response. In H.D. Kimmel, E.H. van Olst, G.F. Orlebeke (Eds.), The orienting reflex in humans (pp. 499–519 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach, E. Luzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex, 14, 129–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach, E., Rizzolatti, G. (Eds.) (1987). Visual selective attention. Neuropsychologic 25. ( Special issue).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach, E., Luzzatti, C., Perani, D. (1979). Unilateral neglect, representational schema and con–sciousness. Brain, 102, 609–618.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach, E., Berti, A., Vallar, G. (1985). Analogical and logical disorders underlying unilateral ne–glect of space. In M.I. Posner O.S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance 11 (pp. 239–249 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach, E., Capitani, E., Porta, E. (1985). Two properties of space representation in the brain: Evi–dence from unilateral neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 48, 141–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach, E., Capitani, E., Luzzatti, C., Perani, D. (1981). Brain and conscious representation of outside reality. Neuropsychologia, 19, 543–551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boden, M.A. (1988). Artificial intelligence and biological intelligence. In H.J. Jerison I. Jerison (Eds.), Intelligence and evolutionary biology (pp. 45–71 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B., Kirch, M., Sperling, A. (1981). Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion. Perception Psychophysics, 29, 336–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D.E. (1958). Perception and communication. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D.E. (1971). Decision and stress. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D.E. (1982). Task combination and selective intake of information. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam), 50, 253–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, C.J., Goldberg, M.F. (1984). Physiology of the frontal eye fields. Trends in Neuroscience, 7, 436–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bushnell, M.C., Goldberg, M.E., Robinson, D.L. (1981). Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex: I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to selective visual attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 46, 755–772.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butter, C.M. (1987). Varieties of attention and disturbances of attention: A neuropsychological analysis. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect (pp. 1–23 ). Amsterdam: Elseviers North–Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheesman, J., Merikle, P.M. (1985). Word recognition and consciousness. In D. Besner, T.G. Waller, G.E. MacKinnon (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 311–352 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowey, A. (1979). Cortical maps in visual perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 1–17.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. (1984). The function of the thalamic reticular complex: The searchlight hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 81, 4586–4590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D.P. (1983). The frontal eye field and attention. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 232–260.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csanyi, V. (1988). Contribution of the genetic and neural memory to animal intelligence. In H.J. Jerison I. Jerison (Eds.), Intelligence and evolutionary biology (pp. 299–318 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dark, V., Johnston, W.A., Myles–Worsley, M., Farah, M.J. (1985). Levels of selection and capacity limits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 472–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Renzi, E. (1982). Disorders of space exploration and cognition. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R., Schein, S.J., Moran, J., Ungerleider, L. (1985). Contour, color and shape analysis beyond the striate cortex. Vision Research, 25, 441–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, J.A., Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70, 80–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downing, C.J. Pinker, S. (1985). The spatial structure of visual attention. In M.I. Posner O.S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance 11 (pp. 171–187 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eason, R.G. (1984). Selective attention effects on retinal and forebrain responses in humans: A replication and extension. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22, 341–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eason, R.G., Oakley, M., Rowers, L. (1983). Central neural influences on the human retina during selective attention. Physiological Psychology, 11, 18–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C.W., Hoffman, J.E. (1973). The extent of processing of noise elements during selective encoding from visual displays. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 155–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C.W., Hoffman, J.E. (1974). Selective attention: Noise suppression or signal enhancement? Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 587–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C.W., Yeh, Y.–Y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 583–597.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J.A. (1985). Four frames suffice: A provisional model of vision and space. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 265–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, B. (1986). The role of attention in the preparation of visually guided eye movements in monkey and man. Psychological Research, 48, 251–257.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M.S. (1987). Perceptual and attentional processes following callosal section in humans. Neuropsychology, 25, 119–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J.J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, M.E., Bruce, C.J. (1985). Cerebral cortical activity associated with the orientation of visual attention in the rhesus monkey. Vision Research, 25, 471–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, J.D. (1976). Looking at pictures. In R.A. Monty G.W. Senders (Eds.), Eye movements and psychological processes (pp. 323–345 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, M.R., Aine, C.J. (1984). Brain mechanisms of visual selective attention. In R. Parasuraman D.R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 293–321 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, M.R., Aine, C., Schroeder, C. (1982). Hemispheric differences in neural processing of stimulus location and type: Effects of selective attention on visual evoked potentials. Neuropsycholo– gia, 20, 421–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K.M., Valenstein, E. (1979). Mechanisms underlying hemispheric neglect. Archives of Neurology, 5, 166–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K.M., Valenstein, E. (1985). The neglect syndrome. In J.A.M. Frederiks (Ed.), Handbook of clinical neurology: Vol 1 (45). Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillyard, S.A., Münte, T.F. (1984). Selective attention to color and location: An analysis with event– related brain potentials. Perception Psychophysics, 36, 185–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillyard, S.A., Münte, T.F., Neville, H.J. (1985). Visual–spatial attention, orienting and brain physiology. In M.I. Posner O.S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance 11 (pp. 63–84 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinde, R.A. (1970). Animal behavior: A synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology. New York: McGraw–Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, W., Kalmar, D. (1987). Characterizing attentional resources. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 68–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodos, W. (1988). Comparative neuroanatomy and the evolution of intelligence. In H.J. Jerison I. Jerison (Eds.), Intelligence and evolutionary biology (pp. 299–318 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J.E. (1986). Spatial attention in vision. Evidence for early selection. Psychological Research, 48, 221–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J.E., MacMillan, F.W. (1985). Is semantic priming automatic?. In M.I. Posner O.S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance 11 (pp. 585–599 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofman, M.A. (1988). Brain, mind, and reality: An evolutionary approach to biological intelligence. In H.J. Jerison I. Jerison (Eds.), Intelligence and evolutionary biology (pp. 299–318 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holtzman, J.D., Sidtis, J.J., Volpe, B.T., Wilson, D.H., Gazzaniga, M.S. (1981). Dissociation of spatial information for stimulus localization and the control of attention. Brain, 104, 861–872.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holtzman, J.D., Volpe, B.T., Gazzaniga, M.S. (1984). Spatial orientation following commissural section. In R. Parasuraman D.R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 375–394 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, H.C., Zimba, L.D. (1987). Natural boundaries for the spatial spread of directed visual attention. Neuropsychology, 25, 5–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G.W., Evett, L.J., Taylor, D.E. (1982). Automatic phonological priming in visual word recognition. Memory Cognition, 10, 576–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurlbert, A., Poggio, T. (1985). Spotlight on attention. Trends inNeuroscience, 8, 309–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyvärinen, J. (1982). The parietal cortex of monkey and man. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingle, D. (1975). Focal attention in the frog: Behavioral and physiological correlates. Science, 188, 1033–1035.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (Ed.) (1987). Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. Amsterdam: Elsevier North–Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jerison, H.J. (1973). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jerison, H.J. (1982). The evolution of biological intelligence. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of human intelligence (pp. 723–791 ). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jerison, H.J. (1988). Evolutionary biology of intelligence: The nature of the problem. In H.J. Jerison I. Jerison (Eds.), Intelligence and evolutionary biology (pp. 1–11 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, W.A., Dark, V. (1985). Dissociable domains of selective processing. In M.I. Posner O.S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance 11 (pp. 567–583 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaas, J.H. (1987). The organization of neocortex in mammals: Implications for theories of brain function. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 129–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S.W., Neill. W.T. (1978). Mechanisms of attention. In E.C. Carterette M.P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception: Vol. 9* Perceptual processing (pp. 3–47 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, B. (1983). Memory, action and motor control. In R.A. Magill (Ed.), Memory and control of action (pp. 47–65 ). Amsterdam: North–Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimmel, H.D., van Olst, E.H., Orlebeke, J.F. (Eds.). (1979). The orienting reflex in humans. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klopfer, D.S. (1985). Constructing mental representations of objects from successive views. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 566–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, U., Rohr, G. (1982). A model of driver behavior. Ergonomics, 25, 891–907.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kundel, H.L., Nodine, C.F. (1978). Studies of eye movements and visual search in radiology. In J.W. Senders, D.F. Fischer, R.A. Monty (Eds.), Eye movements and the higher psychological functions (pp. 317–328 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. Brown, V. (1989). Theory of attentional operation in shape identification. Psychological Review, 96, 101–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lara, R., Arbib, M.A. (1982). A neural model of interaction between tectum and pretectum in prey selection. Cognition and Brain Theory, 5, 149–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leclerc, C., Freibergs, V. (1971). L influence dindices subliminaux perceptifs et symboliques sur la formation dun concept. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 25, 292–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D.N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time–to–collision. Perception, 5, 437–459.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D.N. (1980). Visuo–motor coordination in space–time. In G.E. Stelmach J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 281–295 ). Amsterdam: North–Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D.N., Aaronson, E. (1974). Visual proprioceptive control of standing in human infants. Perception Psychophysics, 15, 529–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D.N., Lishman, J.R. (1975). Visual proprioceptive control of stance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 1, 87–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D.N., Lishman, R. (1977). Visual control of locomotion. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 18, 224–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D.N., Lishman, R., Thomson, J. A. (1982). Regulation of gait in long jumping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 448–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D.N., Young, D.S. (1986). Gearing action to the environment. In H. Heuer C. Fromm (Eds.), Generation and modulation of action patterns (pp. 217–230 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D.N., Young, D.S., Reddish, P.E., Lough, S., Clayton, T.M.H. (1983). Visual timing in hitting an accelerating ball. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 35, 333–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lennie, P. (1980). Parallel visual pathways: A review. Vision Research, 20 561–594.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lettvin, J.Y., Maturana, H.R., McCulloch, W.S., Pitts, W.H. (1961). What the frogs eye tells the frogs brain. In W.R. Rosenblith (Ed.), Sensory communication (pp. 757–776 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, G.R. (1976). A framework for a theory of picture recognition. In Monty R.A., Sanders, J.W. (Eds.), Eye movements and psychological processes (pp. 499–513 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, J.C. (1980). The functional organization of the posterior parietal association cortex. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 485–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, R. (1966). Attention, arousal and the orienting reaction. New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D.M. (1978). The dynamics of perception. In P.A. Buser A. Rougeoul–Buser (Eds.), Cerebral correlates of conscious experience (pp. 53–68 ). Amsterdam: North–Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D.M. (1984). Evaluation–The missing link between perception and action. In W. Prinz A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and motor processes (pp. 175–184 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackworth, N.H., Morandi, A.J. (1967). The gaze selects informative details within pictures. Per–ception Psychophysics, 2, 547–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A.J. (1983a). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A.J. (1983b) Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 238–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., Rumelhart, D.E. (Eds.). (1987). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the structure of cognition: Vol. 2. Psychological and biological models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, P., McLaughlin, C., Nimmo–Smith, J. (1985). Information encapsulation and automaticity: Evidence from the visual control of finely timed actions. In M.I. Posner O.S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance 11 (pp. 391–407 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metzger, W. (1954). Psychologie. Darmstadt: Steinkopff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mishkin, G., Ungerleider, L.G., Macko, K.A. (1983). Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways. Trends in Neuroscience, 6, 414–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Möhler, C.W., Wurtz, R.H. (1976). Organization of monkey superior colliculus: Intermediate layer cells discharging before eye movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 39, 722–744.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran, J., Desimone, R. (1985). Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science, 229, 782–784.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V.B. (1975). The world around us: Neural command functions for selective attention. Neuroscience Research Progress Bulletin (Suppl. 14 ), 1–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V.B. (1978). Brain mechanisms for directed attention. Journal of the Royal Society of Me–dicine, 71, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V.B., Lynch, J.C., Georgopoulos, A., Sakata, H., Acuna, C. (1975). Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: Command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. Journal of Neurophysiology, 38, 871–908.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, H.J., Findlay, J. (1987). Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial cuing of visual atten–tion. Perception Psychophysics, 42, 383–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müsseier, J. (1987). Aufmerksamkeitsverlagerungen und Relativität. Ein experimenteller Beitrag zur Raum–Zeit–Wahrnehmung anhand eines Kontraktionsphänomens (Tandem Effekt) [Attention shifts and relativity. An experimental contribution to the perception of space and time, using a contraction phenomenon (Tandem Effect)]. Munich: Minerva.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müsseier, J. Neumann, O. (submitted). Apparent distance reduction with moving stimuli (Tandem Effect): Evidence for an attentional interpretation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D., Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual task interference. Journal of Experi–mental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 435–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1978a). Zum Mechanismus der Interferenz beim dichotischen Hören [On the mechanism of interference in dichotic listening]. Reports from the Cognitive Psychology Unit, Ruhr University Bochum, No. 5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1978b). Aufmerksamkeit als zentrale Verarbeitungskapazität Anmerkungen zu einer Metapher [Attention as central processing capacity Remarks on a metaphor]. In M. Tücke W. Deffner (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd Osnabrück Psychology Workshop. Osnabrück: University of Osnabrück.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1980). Informationsselektion und Handlungssteuerung. Untersuchungen zur Funktionsgrundlage des Stroop–Interferenzphänomens [Information selection and action control. Studies on

    Google Scholar 

  • the mechanisms underlying the Stroop interference phenomenon]. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Ruhr University Bochum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1983). Über den Zusammenhang zwischen Enge und Selektivität der Aufmerksamkeit [On the relation between the limits and the selectivity of attention]. Reports from the Cognitive Psychology Unit, Ruhr University Bochum, No. 19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1984). Automatic processing: A review of recent findings and a plea for an old theoiy. In W. Prinz A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and motor processes (pp. 255–293 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1985). Die Hypothese begrenzter Kapazität und die Funktionen der Aufmerksamkeit (The limited capacity hypothesis and the functions of attention). In O. Neumann (Ed.), Perspektiven der Kognitionspsychologie (pp. 185–229 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1987a). Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention. In H. Heuer A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 361–394 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1987b). Zur Funktion der selektiven Aufmerksamkeit für die Handlungssteuerung [On the functions of selective attention for action control]. Sprache Kognition, 6, 107–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1987c). An evaluation of three concepts of consciousness. Reports from the Research Group on Perception and Action at the Center for interdisciplinary Research (ZiF), University of Bielefeld, No. 150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1989). Kognitive Vermittlung und direkte Parameterspezifikation. Zum Problem mentaler Repräsentation in der Wahrnehmung [Cognitive mediation and direct parameter specification: On the problem of mental representation in perception]. Sprache Kognition, 8, 32–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O., van der Heijden, A.H.C., Allport, D.A. (1986a). Visual selective attention: Introductory remarks. Psychological Research, 48, 185–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O., van der Heijden, A.H.C., Allport, D.A. (Eds.) (1986b). Visual selective attention (Psychological Research special issue). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parks, T. (1965). Post–retinal visual storage. American Journal of Psychology, 78, 145–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, S.E., Robinson, D.L., Morris, J.D. (1987). Contributions of the pulvinar to visual spatial attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 97–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I., Nissen, M.J., Ogden, W.C. (1978). Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. In H.I. Pick E. Saltzman (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information (pp. 137–157 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I., Snyder, C.R.R., Davidson, B.J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I., Walker, J.A., Friedrich, F.J., Rafal, R.D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. Journal ofNeuroscience, 4, 1863–1874.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I., Walker, J.A., Friedrich, F.A. Rafal, R.D. (1987). How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? Neuropsychologia, 25, 135–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pribram, K.H. (1979). The orienting reaction: Key to brain representational mechanisms? In H.D. Kimmel, E.H. van Olst, G.F. Orlebeke (Eds.) The orienting reflex in humans. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reeves, A., Sperling, G.A. (1986). Attentional gating in short–term visual memory. Psychological Review, 93, 180–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ribot, T. (1906). Psychologie de lattention (Psychology of attention). Paris: Alcan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G. (1983). Mechanisms of selective attention in mammals. In J.–P. Ewert, R.R. Capranica, D.J. Ingle (Eds.), Advances in vertebrate neuroethology (pp. 261–297 ). London: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R. (1987). Neural circuits for spatial attention and unilateral neglect. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect (pp. 289– 313 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier North–Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Grupp, L.A., Pisa, M. (1974). Inhibitory effect of remote visual stimuli on the visual responses of the cat superior colliculus: Spatial and temporal factors. Journal of Neurophysiology, 37, 1262–1275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Gentilucci, M., Matelli, M. (1985). Selective spatial attention: One center, one circuit, or many circuits? In M.I. Posner O.S. Marin (Eds.), Attention and performance 11 (pp. 251–265 ). HiHsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., Umilta, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D.L., Petersen, S.E. (1986). The neurobiology of attention. In J. LeDoux W. Hirst (Eds.), Brain and mind (pp. 142–171 ). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D.L., Goldberg, M.E., Stanton, G.B. (1978). Parietal association cortex in the primate: Sensory mechanisms and behavioral modulations. Journal of Neurophysiology, 41, 910–932.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I., Halper, F., DiViat, J., Wheeler, D. (1987). Eye movement as a cue to figure motion in an– orthoscopic perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 344–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrbaugh, J.W. (1984). The orienting reflex: Performance and central system manifestations. In R. Pa– rasuraman D.R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 323–373 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rugg, M.D., Milner, A.D., Lines, C.R., Phalp, R. (1987). Modulation of visual event–related potentials by spatial and non–spatial selective attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 85–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E., McClelland, J.L. (Eds.). (1987). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the structure of cognition: Vol. 1. Foundations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saltzman, E.L. (1979). Levels of sensorimotor representation. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 20, 91–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1987). Connectionism: Is it a paradigm shift for psychology? Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 19, 73–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1972). The functions of consciousness. Psychological Review, 79, 383–393.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1978). The dominant action system: An information–processing approach to consciousness. In K.S. Pope J.L. Singer (Eds.), The stream of consciousness (pp. 117–157 ). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd, M., Müller, H.H. (1989). Movement versus focusing of visual attention. Perception and Psychophysics, 46, 146–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shor, R.E. (1969). Hypnosis and the concept of generalized reality–orientation. In C.E. Tart (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness (pp. 233–261 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, G.L., Remington, R.W., McLean, J.P. (1979). Moving attention through visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 522–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, G.L., Sheehy, J.B., Wilson, J. (1986). Gradients of spatial attention. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam), 61, 167–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1969). The science of the artificial. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E.N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E.N. (1975). The neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex. In E.N. Sokolov O.S. Vino– gradova (Eds.), Neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex (pp. 217–235 ). Hillsdale, Nj: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E.N., Vinogradova, O.S. (Eds.). (1975). Neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G. (1963). A model for visual memory tasks. Human Factors, 5, 19–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffer, T.H. (1988). Dynamische Aspekte der visuellen Aufmerksamkeit [Dynamic aspects of visual attention!. Unpublished Thesis, Bielefeld: University of Bielefeld.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroud, J. (1949). The psychological moment in perception. In H. v. Foerster (Ed.), Transactions of the sixth conference on cybernetics (pp. 27–63 ). New York: Josiah Macy Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tassinari, G., Aglioti, S., Chelazzi, L. Marzi, C.A., Berlucchi, G. (1987). Distribution in the visual field of the costs of voluntarily allocated attention and of the inhibitory aftereffects of covert orienting. Neuropsychologia, 25, 55–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thistlethwaite, D. (1951). A critical review of latent learning and related experiments. Psychological Bulletin, 48, 97–129.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E.C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189–208.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., Geffen, G. (1967). Selective attention: Perception or response? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 1–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., Gelade, G. (1980). A feature–integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C.B. (1978). Modes of perceiving and modes of acting. In H.I. Pick E. Saltzman (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information (pp. 99–136 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsal, Y. (1983). Movements of attention across the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 523–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Umiltà, C. (1988). Orienting of attention. In F. Boiler J. Grafman (Eds.) Handbook of neuropsycho– logy (pp. 175–193 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, L.G., Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D.J. Ingle, M.A. Goodale, R.J.W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Heijden, A.H.C. (1986). On selection in vision. Psychological Research, 48, 211–219.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Heijden, A.H.C. (1987). Central selection in vision. In H. Heuer A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Per–spectives on perception and action (pp. 421–446 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Heijden, A.H.C., Eerland, E. (1973). The effects of cueing in a visual signal detection task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 496–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Heijden, A.H.C., Wolters, G., Groep, J.C., Hagenaar, R. (1987). Single–letter recognition accuracy benefits from advance cuing of location. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 503–509.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Essen, D.C., Maunsell, H.R. (1983). Hierarchical organization and functional streams in the visual cortex. Trends in Neuroscience, 6, 370–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Olst, E.H., Heemstra, M.L., ten Kortenaar, T. (1979). Stimulus significance and the orienting reaction. In H.D. Kimmel, E.H. van Olst, J.F. Orlebeke (Eds.), The orienting reflex in humans (pp. 521–547 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vierordt, K. (1968). Der Zeitsinn nach Versuchen (The time sense according to experiments). Tübingen: Laupp.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Helmholtz, H. (1866) Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik. (Handbook of Physiological Optics). Leipzig: Voss.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, E.A., Friedland, R. (Eds.). (1977). Hemi–inattention and hemispheric specialization. New York: Raven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. (1977). Entnahme der Identitäts– und Positionsinformation bei der Identifikation tachistosko– pischer Buchstabenzeilen. Ein theoretischer und experimenteller Beitrag zur Grundlagenforschung des Lesens [Extraction of identity and position information during the identification of tachistosco– pically presented letter rows. A theoretical and experimental contribution to the investigation of basic processes in reading]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ruhr University Bochum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. (1984). Saccadic eye movements and visual stability: Preliminary considerations towards a cognitive approach. In W. Prinz A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and motor processes (pp. 121– 137 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1903). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie (Fundamentals of Physiological Psychology) ( 5th Ed. ). Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wurtz, R.H., Goldberg, M.E., Robinson, D.L. (1980). Behavioral modulation of visual responses in the monkey: Stimulus selection for attention and movement. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, 9, 43–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wurtz, R.H., Goldberg, M.E. Robinson, D.L. (1982). Brain mechanisms of visual attention. Scientific American, 246 (6), 124–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yarbus, A.L. (1967). Eye movements and vision. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yates, J. (1985). The content of awareness is a model of the world. Psychological Review, 92, 249–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimba, L.D., Hughes, H.C. (1987). Distractor target interactions during directed visual attention. Spatial Vision, 2, 117–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zöllner, F. (1862). Über eine neue Art anorthoskopischer Zerrbilder [On a new kind of anorthoscopic distorted pictures]. Poggendorfs Annalen der Physik, 117, 477–484.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1990 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Neumann, O. (1990). Visual Attention and Action. In: Neumann, O., Prinz, W. (eds) Relationships Between Perception and Action. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-75348-0_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-75348-0_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-75350-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-75348-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics