Skip to main content

Animal Timing

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Psychology of Time Perception
  • 3076 Accesses

Abstract

The importance of animal timing in the historical development of modern accounts of time perception cannot be overestimated, even if interest in animal timing per se has been declining for two decades or more. In this chapter, I attempt to trace some of the most interesting and influential research initially carried out in non-human animals, and to look at some ingenious and provocative theoretical models of animal performance on timing tasks, in particular competitors to SET. Animal learning, of which much of the work on animal timing is an offshoot, at one time occupied a central place in academic psychology, but now may be taught in only a cursory manner, if at all, in the undergraduate curriculum of many psychology departments, at least in the UK. This means that contemporary readers may be much less familiar with methods used and ideas derived from Pavlovian and operant conditioning. I cannot provide anything close to an in-depth course in animal learning in a volume on time perception, so a book like Domjan (1993, or later editions) should be consulted for background. I will, however, try to provide the reader with sufficient detail to enable an understanding of the results of the studies I describe. For simplicity of exposition, I have given myself licence to simplify, or even slightly misrepresent, the often complex techniques used in experiments with animals. In particular, some procedures require lengthy pre-training, with gradual approximations to the final method used. I will generally omit any mention of this pre-training, but I will sometimes present simplified versions of the techniques used.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Arantes, J., & Machado, A. (2008). Context effects in a temporal discrimination task: Further tests of the scalar expectancy theory and learning-to-time models. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 90, 33–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1970). Reinforcement schedules and psychophysical judgments. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules (pp. 1–42). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M., & Deluty, M. Z. (1977). Bisection of temporal intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3, 216–228.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M., & Gibbon, J. (1982). Temporal generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, 165–186.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M., Meck, W. H., & Gibbon, J. (1994). The application of scalar timing theory to individual trials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 135–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Domjan, M. (1993). The principles of learning and behavior. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Droit-Volet, S., Clément, A., & Fayol, M. (2003). Time and number discrimination in a bisection task with a sequence of stimuli: A developmental approach. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 63–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Droit-Volet, S., Clément, A., & Wearden, J. H. (2001). Temporal generalization in 3- to 8-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 271–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fetterman, J. G., & Killeen, P. R. (1995). Categorical scaling of time: Implications for clock-counter models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 43–63.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s Law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J. (1979). Timing the stimulus and the response in aversive control. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.), Reinforcement and the organisation of behaviour (pp. 299–340). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J. (1981). On the form and location of the psychometric bisection function for time. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 24, 58–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J., & Church, R. M. (1990). Representation of time. Cognition, 37, 23–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Timing and time perception, Vol. 423, pp. 52–77). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouvea, T. S., Monteiro, T., Soares, S., Atallah, B. V., & Paton, J. J. (2014). Ongoing behaviour predicts perceptual report interval duration. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 8, 10.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Haight, P. A., & Killeen, P. R. (1991). Adjunctive behaviour in multiple schedules of reinforcement. Animal Learning and Behavior, 19, 257–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jasselette, P., Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (1990). The perching response and the laws of animal timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 150–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jozefowiez, J., Polack, C. W., Machado, A., & Miller, R. R. (2014). Trial frequency effects in human temporal bisection: Implications for theories of timing. Behavioural Processes, 101, 81–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jozefowiez, J., Staddon, J. E. R., & Cerutti, D. T. (2009). The behavioural economics of choice and interval timing. Psychological Review, 116, 519–539.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Killeen, P. R., & Fetterman, J. G. (1988). A behavioral theory of timing. Psychological Review, 95, 274–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, G., & Rilling, G. M. (1970). Differential reinforcement of low rates: A selective critique. Psychological Bulletin, 74, 225–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lejeune, H., Cornet, S., Ferreira, J., & Wearden, J. H. (1998). How do Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) pass the time? Adjunctive behavior during temporal differentiation in gerbils. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 24, 325–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lejeune, H., & Jasselette, P. (1986). Accurate DRL performance in the pigeon: Comparison between perching and treadle pressing. Animal Learning and Behavior, 14, 205–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lejeune, H., Richelle, M., & Wearden, J. H. (2006). About Skinner and time: Behavior-analytic contributions to research on animal timing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 125–142.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (1991). The comparative psychology of fixed-interval responding: Some quantitative analyses. Learning and Motivation, 22, 84–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (2006). Scalar properties in animal timing: Conformity and violations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1875–1908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., Harzem, P., & Spencer, P. T. (1979). Temporal control of behavior and the power law. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 333–344.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Machado, A. (1997). Learning the temporal dynamics of behavior. Psychological Review, 104, 241–265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Machado, A., & Keen, R. (1999). Learning to Time (LET) or Scalar Expectancy Theory (SET)? A critical test of two models of timing. Psychological Science, 10, 285–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machado, A., Malheiro, M. T., & Erlhagen, W. (2009). Learning to time: A perspective. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 92, 423–458.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 139–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meck, W. H. (1983). Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 171–201.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1960). Conditioned reflexes. New York: Dover Publications. Original edition 1927.

    Google Scholar 

  • Platt, J. R. (1979). Temporal differentiation and the psychophysics of time. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem (Eds.), Reinforcement and the organisation of behaviour (pp. 1–29). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1991). Associative relations in instrumental learning: The eighteenth Bartlett Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B, 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richelle, M., & Lejeune, H. (1980). Time in animal behaviour. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, S. (1981). Isolation of an internal clock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 242–268.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R., & Ayres, S. (1975). Sequential and temporal properties of behaviour induced by a schedule of periodic food delivery. Behaviour, 54, 26–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R., & Higa, J. (1999). Time and memory: Towards a pacemaker-free theory of interval timing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 215–251.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R., & Simmelhag, V. L. (1971). The “superstition” experiment: A re-examination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review, 78, 3–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wearden, J. H. (1985). The power law and Weber’s law in fixed-interval postreinforcement pausing: A scalar timing model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37B, 191–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wearden, J. H. (1990). Maximizing reinforcement rate on spaced responding schedules under conditions of temporal uncertainty. Behavioural Processes, 22, 47–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wearden, J. H. (1991b). Human performance on an analogue of an interval bisection task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43B, 59–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wearden, J. H., & Lejeune, H. (2006). “The stone which the builders rejected”: Delay of reinforcement and response rate on fixed-interval and related schedules. Behavioural Processes, 71, 77–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitaker, J. S., Lowe, C. F., & Wearden, J. H. (2003). Multiple-interval timing in rats: Performance on two-valued mixed fixed-interval schedules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 29, 277–291.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitaker, S., Lowe, C. F., & Wearden, J. H. (2008). When to respond? And how much? Temporal control and response output on mixed-fixed-interval schedules with unequally-probable components. Behavioural Processes, 77, 33–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Appendix: Correlations Between Performance Measures from the Peak Procedure

Appendix: Correlations Between Performance Measures from the Peak Procedure

The aim of the arguments advanced in Gibbon and Church (1990) and Church et al. (1994) was to determine the contribution of different sources of variance to performance on individual peak procedure trials. In SET, there can be trial-to-trial variance in both memory/timing and the threshold for starting and stopping responding, as discussed in Chap. 3 and earlier in this chapter. A simplified outline of how these might contribute to performance is given here, but for full mathematical exposition, the reader should consult the articles mentioned immediately above.

In any attempt to assess the contribution of different variance sources, the data of interest are the correlations between the different performance measures (mainly start, stop, and spread). Gibbon and Church (1990) and Church et al. (1994; see their Table 2, p. 146) characterize the correlation pattern as follows, in which the values given are the means from Church et al.’s own study. Start and stop values are positively correlated (mean value 0.31), whereas start and spread (the length of the response period) are negatively correlated (mean −0.33).

Equation (9.1), the usual SET rule for judging the similarity of two time values, determines the start time and the stop time. Suppose that S is the standard FI value of the peak procedure task. The start of responding (ST) occurs when

$$ \left(S\hbox{--} ST\right)/S < b $$

Or, rearranging, ST = S − Sb

And the stop (SP) occurs when

$$ \left(SP\hbox{--} S\right)/S > b,\mathrm{or}SP = S + Sb $$

where b is the threshold.

We consider first the case where there is only trial-by-trial variability in S, and b remains constant. Suppose that the FI value in the peak procedure is 60 s, and the threshold is 0.25. Thus, ST = 45 s, SP = 75 s, and the spread is 75 − 45 = 30 s. The case just discussed assumes that the FI value is represented correctly, but SET proposes the existence of memory/timing variance, so on another trial, the value sampled from the memory might be “short,” 50 s. Now, SP = 48.5 s, ST = 62.5 s, and the spread is 24 s. On another trial, a “long” 70-s value might be sampled, so ST = 53.5 s, SP = 87.5 s, and the spread is 34 s. From these three examples, we can see that “early” starts lead to “early” stops, and “late” starts lead to “late” stops, so ST and SP are positively correlated. However, ST and the spread are also positively correlated: a “late” start gives rise to a longer response period than an “early” start. In data, ST and SP are positively correlated, but ST and the spread are negatively correlated. This shows that an account with memory/timing variance alone cannot produce the full pattern of correlations obtained.

Now, consider the effect of trial-by-trial variability only in the threshold, b, with S constant at 60 s on each trial. If b = 0.25, we have the case above with ST = 45 s, SP = 75 s, and spread = 30 s. If the threshold is more conservative (b = 0.2), then ST is 48 s, SP is 72 s, and the spread is 24 s. This is intuitively reasonable: if the decision to start and stop responding is more conservative, the response period should start later and finish earlier. In contrast, if b = 0.3, then ST = 42 s, SP = 78 s, and the spread is 36 s. Thus, when the threshold alone is varied, early starts lead to late stops, and late starts to early stops, so ST and SP are negatively correlated. However, the examples also show that early starts lead to longer response periods (spreads) than later starts: in other words, start and spread are negatively correlated. By itself, this model does not fit overall correlation patterns, as ST and SP are positively correlated in data.

The obvious suggestion that can be derived from the outline above is that an account with both memory/timing variance and threshold variance is needed to model the pattern of correlations in data, and this is exactly what Gibbon and Church (1990) and Church et al. (1994) propose. An additional issue is the number of samples of both memory and threshold used in a single trial. For example, the animal might sample the reference memory once and the threshold once, so the same threshold is used for start and stop decisions. Alternatively, the reference memory might be sampled twice (once for the start decision and once for the stop decision), and likewise, the threshold might be sampled twice for the same decisions. The arguments are too complex to illustrate here, but the articles cited above suggest that a single memory sample per trial is used, with either one threshold or different thresholds for start and stop.

Copyright information

© 2016 The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wearden, J. (2016). Animal Timing. In: The Psychology of Time Perception. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-40883-9_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics