Skip to main content

Closure

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:
Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology
  • 2 Accesses

Synonyms

Visual integration; Visual synthesis

Definition

Visual closure refers to the ability to perceive and recognize objects, shapers, features, or symbols from incomplete or degraded visual stimuli. It reflects the capacity of humans to fill in missing information from incomplete sensory input to achieve a meaningful percept.

Historical Perspective

The principle of visual closure had its roots in Gestalt psychology (Ellis 1938; Harlow 1938; Köhler 1929). Gestalt psychology theorized that operationally brain functions (i.e., perception and cognition) are holistic consisting of analog processes that occur in a parallel manner and are self-organizing. This led to the well-known conclusion regarding perception, and cognition more generally, that “the whole is greater than the sum of its parts.” Based on this framework, all perceptual processes act to achieve optimal organization and reconciliation with the objects that are being perceived. A critical principle driving Gestalt...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 899.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 1,099.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References and Readings

  • Ellis, W. D. (1938). A source book of Gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Foreman, N. (1991). Correlates of performance on the Gollin and Mooney tests of visual closure. Journal of General Psychology, 118(1), 13–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, R. F. (1938). Philosophy’s contribution to Gestalt psychology. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 5, 185–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, D. S. (1968). Search for “closure” in a visually perceived pattern. Psychological Bulletin, 70(5), 296–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. C., & Dennis, M. E. (1972). Perceptual closure as a function of gap size. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 35(1), 126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology. New York: H. Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mooney, C. M., & Ferguson, G. A. (1951). A new closure test. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 5(3), 129–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poppelreuter, W. (1990). Disturbances of lower and higher visual capacities caused by occipital damage: With special reference to the psychopathological, pedagogical, industrial, and social implications. Oxford/New York: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ronald A. Cohen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this entry

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this entry

Cohen, R.A. (2018). Closure. In: Kreutzer, J.S., DeLuca, J., Caplan, B. (eds) Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57111-9_1352

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics