Skip to main content

Anticipating the Effects of Marketing Communication: A Neuroeconomic Framework for Marketing Purposes

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
Handbuch Sozialtechniken der Kommunikation

Part of the book series: Springer NachschlageWissen ((SRW))

Abstract

High expectations in neuroeconomics raised the hope of marketers that their (daily business) problems could be solved easily. In fact, neuroeconomics has provided considerable insights for marketing science and business practice concerning consumer decision making over the last two decades. However, tapping into customer’s black box of unconscious and automatic processes, so-called implicit processes, does not require the mandatory usage of advanced neuroimaging techniques, such as fMRI. In order to obtain a specific brand positioning in customer’s head, brand communication is an effective means for the marketing of ideas to gain a promoted cortical representation probability and, consequently, an enhanced perceptual and behavioral impact. The marketing of ideas itself is closely related to the concept of brand associations. Those types of associations can be assessed by implicit association measures from psychology (e.g., Implicit Association Test) comparatively accurately but are less properly measured with neuroimaging due to physiological and scientific fallibility. Against this background, the current work introduces a practical neuroscience-related brand communication framework incorporating explicit and implicit brand-related associations to provide guidance for serious marketing-related communication purposes. Moreover, the performance of the introduced conceptual model is tested. In detail, the presented case study demonstrated sufficient performance to detect associative branding change via a (short) communication contact. Specifically, the combined application of implicit, concretely an advanced latency-based tool, and explicit measures, concretely a conventional self-report, provides an integrated assessment of brand-related marketing efforts in general and brand communication activities in particular.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aaker, J. L. (2000). Accessibility or diagnosticity? Disentangling the influence of culture on persuasion processes and attitudes. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(4), 340–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aaker, D. A., Bagozzi, R. P., Carman, J. M., & MacLachlan, J. M. (1980). On using response latency to measure preference. Journal of Marketing Research, 17(2), 237–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aguirre, G. K. (2014). Functional neuroimaging: Technical, logical, and social perspectives. Hastings Center Report, 44(2), 8–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariely, D., & Berns, G. S. (2010). Neuromarketing: The hope and hype of neuroimaging in business. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(4), 284–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J., Ramsøy, T. Z., & Laureys, S. (2003). Brain, conscious experience and the observing self. Trends in Neurosciences, 26(12), 671–675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrouillet, P., Portrat, S., & Camos, V. (2011). On the law relating processing to storage in working memory. Psychological Review, 118(2), 175–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, M. S., Movellan, J. R., Littlewort, G. C., Braathen, B., Frank, M. G., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2005). Toward automatic recognition of spontaneous facial actions. In P. Ekman & E. L. Rosenberg (eds.), What the face reveals: Basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS) (2nd edition, pp. 393–412). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bednarik, R., & Tukiainen, M. (2007). Validating the Restricted Focus Viewer: A study using eye-movement tracking. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 274–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, C. M., Baird, A. A., Miller, M. B., & Wolford, G. L. (2010). Neural correlates of interspecies perspective taking in the post-mortem Atlantic Salmon: An argument for multiple comparisons correction. Journal of Serendipitous and Unexpected Results, 1(1), 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernacer, J., & Murillo, J. I. (2014). The Aristotelian conception of habit and its contribution to human neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bischof, N. (1975). A systems approach toward the functional connections of attachment and fear. Child Development, 46(4), 801–817.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bischof, N. (1985). Das Rätsel Ödipus: Die biologischen Wurzeln des Urkonfliktes von Intimität und Autonomie. München: Piper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bischof, N. (1990). Phase transitions in psychoemotional development. In H. Haken & M. Stadler (eds.), Synergetics of cognition, proceedings of the international symposium at Schloss Elmau, Bavaria, June 4–8 (pp. 361–378). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohner, G., & Dickel, N. (2011). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 391–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., & Pennebacker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 631–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourgine, P. (2004). What is cognitive economics? In O. Bourgine & J.-P. Nadal (eds.), Cognitive economics: An interdisciplinary approach (pp. 1–12). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1984). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In T. C. Kinnear (eds.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 11 pp. 673–675). Provo: Association for Consumer Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1(1), 3–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 752–766.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chicurel, M. (2000). Databasing the brain. Nature, 406, 822–825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choudhury, S., & Slaby, J. (2012). Introduction: Critical neuroscience – between lifeworld and laboratory. In S. Choudhury & J. Slaby (eds.), Critical neuroscience: A handbook of the social and cultural contexts of neuroscience (pp. 1–26). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them? In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (eds.), Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deppe, M., Schwindt, W., Kugel, H., Plassmann, H., & Kenning, P. (2005a). Nonlinear responses within the medial prefrontal cortex reveal when specific implicit information influences economic decision making. Journal of Neuroimaging, 15(2), 171–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deppe, M., Schwindt, W., Krämer, J., Kugel, H., Plassmann, H., Kenning, P., & Ringelstein, E. B. (2005b). Evidence for a neural correlate of a framing effect: Bias-specific activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during credibility judgments. Brain Research Bulletin, 67(5), 413–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: The merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5), 586–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • European Research Council. (2013). ERC projects to unlock mysteries of the human brain. http://erc.europa.eu/sites/default/files/publication/files/unlock_mysteries_of_human_brain.pdf. Accessed 21 Nov 2014.

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (1984). Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 75(4), 451–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • eye square. (2014). BrandREACT. http://www.eye-square.com. Accessed 29 May 2014.

  • Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In M. P. Zanna (eds.), Experimental social psychology (Vol. 23 pp. 75–109). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25(5), 603–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimons, G. M., Chartrand, T. L., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2008). Automatic effects of brand exposure on motivated behavior: How apple makes you “think different”. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(1), 21–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Florack, A., Friese, M., & Scarabis, M. (2010). Regulatory focus and reliance on implicit preferences in consumption contexts. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20(2), 193–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frégnac, Y., & Laurent, G. (2014). Where is the brain in the Human Brain Project? Nature, 513, 27–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., & Weiner, B. (1996). Theories and principles of motivation. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 63–84). New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Rudman, L. A., Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., Farnham, S. D., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109(1), 3–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gubler, H., & Bischof, N. (1991). A systems theory perspective. In M. E. Lamb & H. Keller (eds.), Infant development: Perspectives from German-speaking countries (pp. 35–66). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gutnik, L. A., Hakimzada, A. F., Yoskowitz, N. A., & Patel, V. L. (2006). The role of emotion in decision-making: A cognitive neuroeconomic approach towards understanding sexual risk behaviour. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 39(6), 720–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280–1300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubert, M., & Kenning, P. (2008). A current overview of consumer neuroscience. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 7(4–5), 272–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Interbrand (2014). Methodology. http://www.bestglobalbrands.com/2014/methodology. Accessed 07 Dec 2014.

  • Jansen, A. R., Blackwell, A. F., & Marriott, K. (2003). A tool for tracking visual attention: The Restricted Focus Viewer. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(1), 57–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kable, J. W. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience toolkit for the neuroeconomist: A functional overview. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 4(2), 63–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgement and choice – Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 774–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(1), 16–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, K. L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing customer-based brand equity. Journal of Marketing, 57(1), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, K. L. (1996). Brand Equity and Integrated Communication. In. E. Thorson & J. Moore (eds.), Integrated Communication: Synergy of Persuavise Voices (pp. 103–132). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, K. L. (2003). Brand synthesis: The multidimensionality of brand knowledge. Journal of Consumer Research, 29(4), 595–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, K. L., & Lehmann, D. R. (2006). Brands and branding: Research findings and future priorities. Marketing Science, 25(6), 740–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenning, P., & Linzmajer, M. (2011). Consumer neuroscience – An overview of an emerging discipline with implications for consumer policy. Journal of Consumer Protection and Food Safety, 6(1), 111–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B., Rick, S., Wimmer, G. E., Prelec, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron, 53(1), 147–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindstrom, M. (2008). Buyology: Truth and lies about why we buy. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maison, D., Greenwald, A. G., & Bruin, R. H. (2004). Predictive validity of the Implicit Association Test in studies of brands, consumer attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 405–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markram, H., Meier, K., Lippert, T., Grillner, S., Frackowiak, R., Dehaene, S., Knoll, A., Sompolinsky, H., Verstreken, K., DeFelipe, J., Grant, S., Changeux, J.-P., & Sari, A. (2011). Introducing the human brain project. Procedia Computer Science, 7, 39–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., & Montague, P. R. (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar drinks. Neuron, 44(2), 379–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institutes of Health (2014). The brain initiative. http://www.braininitiative.nih.gov/BRAIN-Brochure.pdf. Accessed 21 Nov 2014.

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19(6), 625–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). The implicit association test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In J. A. Bargh (eds.), Automatic processes in social thinking and behavior (pp. 265–292). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohme, R., Matukin, M., & Szczurko, T. (2010). Neurophysiology uncovers secrets of TV commercials. der markt – Journal für Marketing, 49(3–4), 133–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plassmann, H., Kenning, P., Deppe, M., Kugel, H., & Schwindt, W. (2008). How choice ambiguity modulates activity in brain areas representing brand preference: Evidence from consumer neuroscience. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 7(4–5), 360–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plassmann, H., Ramsøy, T. Z., & Milosavljevic, M. (2012). Branding the brain: A critical review and outlook. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(1), 18–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pradeep, A. K. (2010). The buying brain: Secrets for selling to the subconscious mind. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reimann, M., Zaichkowsky, J., Neuhaus, C., Bender, T., & Weber, B. (2010). Aesthetic package design: A behavioral, neural, and psychological investigation. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20(4), 431–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reimann, M., Schilke, O., Weber, B., Neuhaus, C., & Zaichkowsky, J. (2011). Functional magnetic resonance imaging in consumer research: A review and application. Psychology and Marketing, 28(6), 608–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rocha, M. (2014). Financial applications for brand valuation. http://interbrand.com/en/views/37/financial-applications-for-brand-valuation. Accessed 07 Dec 2014.

  • Schmidt, J. B., & Calantone, R. J. (2009). Escalation of commitment during new product development. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 30(2), 103–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, M. (2001). Systems theory of motivational development. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (eds.), International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (pp. 10120–10125). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, E., & Rosemary, P. (1999). Some basic issues regarding dual-process theories from the perspective of cognitive–experiential self-theory. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 462–482). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, C., Monterosso, J., & Montague, P. R. (2012). Neuroeconomics: A bridge for translational research. Biological Psychiatry, 72(2), 87–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3), 278–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyebjee, T. T. (1979). Response time, conflict, and involvement in brand choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 6(3), 295–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walvis, T. H. (2008). Three laws of branding: Neuroscientific foundations of effective brand building. Journal of Brand Management, 16(3), 176–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, B., Rangel, A., Wibral, M., & Falk, A. (2009). The medial prefrontal cortex exhibits money illusion. PNAS, 106(13), 5025–5028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkie, W. (1986). Consumer behaviour. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107(1), 101–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Steffen Schmidt .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Gabler Verlag | Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden GmbH, Wiesbaden

About this entry

Cite this entry

Schmidt, S., Wiedmann, KP., Reiter, P., Kurlbaum, C. (2016). Anticipating the Effects of Marketing Communication: A Neuroeconomic Framework for Marketing Purposes. In: Langner, T., Esch, FR., Bruhn, M. (eds) Handbuch Sozialtechniken der Kommunikation. Springer NachschlageWissen. Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-05262-1_5-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-05262-1_5-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer Gabler, Wiesbaden

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-658-05262-1

  • eBook Packages: Springer Referenz Wirtschaftswissenschaften

Publish with us

Policies and ethics