Skip to main content

The Development of Prosocial Behavior

  • Chapter
Handbook of Social Development

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Developmental Psychology ((PDPS))

Abstract

There have been energetic debates throughout time regarding the socio-moral nature of human interaction. Centuries ago, theologians and philosophers framed the issues in terms of stark contrasts (e.g., humans were born with the capacity for good or evil, and these qualities were either innate or learned). Depending upon one’s point of view, societal influences could be seen either as corrupting innocent youth or as a means by which aggressive instincts could be subdued over time to produce caring and compassion. Such unidimensional views gradually began to give way, with increased recognition of our multifaceted nature. Research in the last half century has significantly advanced knowledge about biological and environmental contributors to prosocial and antisocial behaviors. Here we review research on prosocial behavior, from a developmental perspective, emphasizing mainly work published in the last decade. By prosocial or altruistic behaviors we mean acts that include provision of comfort or sympathy, helping, sharing, cooperation, rescue, protection, and defense. We also review work on emotions and cognitions that often accompany, and are integral features of, prosocial behaviors. This includes children’s empathy or emotional incorporation of the others’ emotional experience, as well as their moral reasoning and cognitive comprehension of others’ internal states and needs (perspective taking, role taking).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Developmental Psychology, 297-305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbuthnot, J., & Gordon, D. A. (1986). Behavioral and cognitive effects of a moral reasoning development intervention for high-risk behavior-disordered adolescents. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 54, 208–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, M. A. (1987). Empathy and related responses in children. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 146–162). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, M. A., & McCoy, S. A. (1989). The relation of distressful childhood experiences and empathy in college undergraduates. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150, 417–426.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, M. A., McMinimy, V., Flouer, G., & Masbad, I. (1987). Adolescent’s evaluations of peers’ motives for helping. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 579–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Griffitt, C. A., Barrientos, S., Brandt, J. R., Spnengelmeyer, P., & Bayly, M. J. (1989). Negative-state relief and the empathy-altruism hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 922–933.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Dyck, J. L., Brandt, J. R., Batson, J. G., Powell, A. L., McMaster, M. R., & Griffitt, C. (1988). Fives studies testing two new egoistic alternatives to the empathy-altruism hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 52–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauman, D. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Kendrick, D. J. (1983). Mood and sex differences in the development of altruism as hedonism. Academic Psychology Bulletin, 5, 299–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1986). Sex differences in moral reasoning: Response to Walker’s (1984) conclusion that there are none. Child Development, 57, 511–521.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bear, G. G. (1989). Sociomoral reasoning and antisocial behaviors around normal sixth graders. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 181–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beardslee, W., & Podorefsky, D. (1988). Resilient adolescents whose parents have serious affective and other psychiatric disorders: Importance of self-understanding and relationships. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145(1), 63–69.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beardslee, W. R., Schultz, L. H., & Selman, R. L. (1987). Level of social-cognitive development, adoptive functioning, and DSM-III diagnoses in adolescent offspring of parents with affective disorders: Implications for the development of the capacity for mutuality. Developmental Psychology, 23(6), 807–815.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bengtsson, H., & Johnson, L. (1987). Cognitions related to empathy in five-to eleven-year-old children. Child Development, 58, 1001–1012.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, T. J. (1981). Effects of friendship on prosocial intentions and behavior. Child Development, 52, 636–643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berthenthal, B., & Fischer, K. W. (1978). The development of self-recognition in the infant. Developmental Psychology, 14, 44–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A critical review of the literature. Psychology Bulletin, 88, 1–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, J., Olweus, D., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1986). Development of anti-social and prosocial behavior: Research, theories and issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Lightbown, P., & Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, serial No. 160, Vol. 40, No. 2. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehnke, K., Silbereisen, R. K., Eisenberg, N., Reykowski, J., & Polomari, A. (1989). Developmental pattern of prosocial motivation: A cross-national study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20, 219–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boom, J., & Molenaar, P C. (1989). A developmental model of hierarchical stage structure in objective moral judgements. Developmental Review, 9, 133–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., Fritz, J., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Ridgeway, D. (1986). The acquisition and development of emotion language: A functionalist perspective. Child Development, 57, 529–548.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody, L. R. (1985). Gender differences in emotional development: A review of theories and research. Journal of Personality, 53, 102–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brothers, L. (1989). A biological perspective on empathy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 10–19.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1972). The nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist, 27, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryan, T., Werner, M., & Pearl, R. (1982). Learning disabled students’ conformity responses to prosocial and antisocial situations. Learning Disability Quarterly, 2(1), 33–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bussey, K., & Maugham, B. (1982). Gender differences in moral reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 701–706.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (1980). A discussion of some issues raised by Piaget’s concept of childhood egocentrism. In M. V. Cox (Ed.), Are young children egocentric? pp. 17–40. York, England: Batsford Academic and Educational.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Stenberg, C. (1983). Sodoemotional Development. In P H. Mussen, M. M. Haith, & J. J. Campos (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Infancy and developmental psychobiology (Vol. 2, pp. 783–915). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, M. & Hay, D. E (1989). Preschoolers’ responses to peers’ distress and beliefs about bystander intervention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 231–242.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, M., & Miller, N. (1987). Explanation of the relation between negative mood and helping. Psychological Bulletin, 102(1), 191–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, J. H., & Ascione, F. R. (1987). The effects of prosocial and aggressive videogames on children’s donating and helping. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 148, 499–505.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M. J. (1973). Egocentric and anti-social behavior: The assessment and training of social perspective-taking skills. Developmental Psychology, 9(3), 326–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, M. J., Greenspan, S., & Barenboim, C. (1974). Assessment and training of role-taking skills in institutionalized emotionally disturbed children. Developmental Psychology, 10(4), 546–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Cooperman, G., & Iannotti, R. (1987). Empathy and responsibility in the motivation of children’s helping. Developmental Psychology, 23, 140–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., Eisenberg, N., Shell, R., & McCreath, H. (1987). Commitments to help by children: Effects on subsequent prosocial self-attributions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 237–245.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., & Kendrick, D. T. (1976). Altruism as hedonism: A social development perspective on the relationship of negative mood and state and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 907–914.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., Schaller, M., Houlihan, D, Arps, K., Fultz, J., & Beaman, A. L. (1987). Empathy-based helping: Is it selflessly motivated? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 749–758.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D. (1990). The organization and coherence of socioemotional, cognitive and representational development: Illustrations through a developmental psychopathology perspective on down syndrome and child maltreatment. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1988: Socioemotional Development, R. Thompson, (Ed.), Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, pp. 259–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clary, E. G., & Miller, J. (1986). Socialization and situational influences on sustained altruism. Child Development, 57, 1358–1369.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, E. M., Hollenbeck, B., Iannotti, R. J., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1986). Early organization of altruism and aggression: Developmental patterns and individual differences. In C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M. Cummings, & R. J. Iannotti (Eds.), Altruism and aggression: Biological and social origins (pp. 165–188) New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1981). Young children’s responses to expressions of anger and affection by others in the family. Child Development, 52, 1274–1282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animal. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denham, S. A., & Couchard, E. A. (1991). Social-emotional predictors of preschoolers’ responses to adult negative emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 595–608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denham, S., McKinley, M., Couchard, E. A., & Holt, R. (1991). Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Development, 61, 1145–1152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derr, A. M. (1986). How learning disabled adolescent boys make moral judgments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 19, 160–164.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donenberg, G. R., & Hoffman, L. W. (1988). Gender differences in moral development. Sex Roles, 18, 701–717.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubow, E. E., Huesmann, L. R., & Eron, L. D. (1987). Mitigating aggression and promoting prosocial behavior in aggressive elementary schoolboys. Behavior Research Theories, 25, 527–531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1986). Siblings and the development of prosocial behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 265–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Crowley, M. (1986). Gender and helping behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 283–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckerman, C. O., Whatley, J. L., & Kutz, S. L. (1975). Growth of social play with peers during the second year of life. Developmental Psychology, 11, 42–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Bustamante, D., Mathy, R. M., Miller, P. A., & Lindholm, E. (1988). Differentiation of vicariously induced emotional reactions in children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 237–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Fabes, P. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, R. M., & Reno, R. R. (1989). Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 55–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 91–119.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Schaller, M., Fabes, R. A., Bustamante, D., Mathy, R. M., Shell, R., & Rhodes, K. (1988). Differentiation of personal distress and sympathy in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 24, 766–775.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., Shell, R., Pasternack, J., Lennon, R., Belber, R., & Matty, R. M. (1987). Prosocial development in middle childhood: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 23, 712–718.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., & Strayer, J. (1987). Empathy and its development. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emde, R., Johnson, W., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1987). The do’s and don’ts of early moral development: Psychoanalytic tradition and current research. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 245–276). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emery, R. E. (1982). Interparent conflict and the children of discord and divorce. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 310–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. (1990). Maternal correlates of children’s vicarious emotional responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 26, 639–648.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabes, R. A., Fultz, J., Eisenberg, N., May-Plumlee, T, & Christopher, F. S. (1989). Effects of rewards on children’s prosocial motivation: A socialization study. Developmental Psychology, 25, 509–515.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fergusion, T. J., & Rule, B. G. (1988). Children’s evaluation of retaliatory aggression. Child Development, 59, 961–968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feshbach, N. D. (1978). Studies of empathie behavior in children. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 8, pp. 1–47), New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feshbach, N. D. (1979). Empathy training: A field study in affective education. In S. Feshbach & A. Fraczek (Eds.), Aggression and behavior change: Biological and social processes (pp. 234–249). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, T., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Francis, L. J., & Pearson, P. R. (1987). Empathic development during adolescence: Religiosity, the missing link? Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 145–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1958). Civilization and its discontents. New York: Doubleday Anchor Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frodi, A., Lamb, M. E., Leavitt, L. A., Donovan, W. A., Neff, C., & Sherry, D. (1978). Fathers’ and mothers’ responses to the faces and cries of normal and premature infants. Developmental Psychology, 14(5), 490–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frodi, A., Macaulay, J., & Thome, P. R. (1977). Are women always less aggressive than men? A review of the experimental literature. Psychological Bulletin, 84(4), 634–660.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. (1977). In a different voice: Women’s conception of self and morality. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 481–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grusec, J. E. (1982). The socialization of altruism. In N. Eisenberg-Berg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 139–166). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grusec, J. E. (1991). Socializing concern for others in the home. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 338–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender differences in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85(4), 845–857.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, D. (1988). A longitudinal study of adolescents’ socialization and identification as predictors of adult moral judgment development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 34, 245–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartshorne, H., May, M. A., & Mahler, J. B. (1929). Studies in the nature of character (Vol. 2), Studies in service and self-control. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haviland, J. M., & Lelivica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: 10-week-old infants’ responses to three emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 97–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haynes-Seman, C. (1987). Developmental origins of moral masochism: A failure-to-thrive toddler’s interactions with mother. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 319–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinshaw, S. P., Henker, B., Whalen, C. K., Erhardt, D, & Dunnighton, R. E. (1989). Aggressive, prosocial, and nonsocial behavior in hyperactive boys: Dose effects of methylphenidate in naturalistic settings. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 57, 636–643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. R (1984). Early childhood autism and the question of egocentrism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 85–104.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Moral development. In P H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology: (Vol. 2) (pp. 261–360). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (1975). Developmental synthesis of affect and cognition and its implications for altruistic motivation. Developmental Psychology, 11, 605–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 712–722.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howes, C., & Eldridge, R. (1985). Responses of abused, neglected and non-maltreated children to the behaviors of their peers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 6, 261–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howes, C., & Farver, J. (1987). Toddler’s responses to the distress of their peers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 8, 441–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudgins, W, & Prentice, N. M. (1973). Moral judgment in delinquent and non-delinquent adolescents and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 82, 145–152.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jurkovic, G. J. (1980). The juvenile delinquent as a moral philosopher: A structural-developmental perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 709–727.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J., & Lamb, S. (1987). The emergence of morality in young children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, P. J., & Arbuthnot, J. (1985). Affective empathy and cognitive role-taking in delinquent and nondeliquent youth. Adolescence, 20, 323–333.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keltikangus-Tarvinen, L. (1989). Moral judgements of aggressive and non-aggressive children. The Journal of Social Psychology, 129, 733–739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klimes-Dougan, B., & Kistner, J. (1990). Physically abused preschoolers’ responses to peers’ distress. Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 599–602.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestner, R., Franz, C., & Weinberger, J. (1990). The family origins of empathie concern: A 26-year longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 709–717.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347–480). New York: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, M., & Prentice, N. M. (1988). Interrelations of empathy, cognition, and moral reasoning with dimensions of juvenile delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 127–139.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lennon, R., & Eisenberg, N. (1987). Emotional displays associated with preschoolers’ prosocial behavior. Child Development, 58, 992–1000.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., Sullivan, M., Stanger, C., & Weiss, M. (1989). Self development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146–156.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, P. D. (1985). Brain evolution relating to family, play, and the separation call. Archives of General Psychology, 42, 405–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Main, M., & George, C. (1985). Response of abused and disadvantaged toddlers to distress in playmates: A study in the day care setting. Developmental Psychology, 21, 407–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984). Predicting rejection of her infant from mother’s representation of her own experience: Implications for the abused-abusing intergenerational cycle. Child Abuse and Neglect, 8, 203–217.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, K. A., Batson, C. D., Horn, J., & Rosenman, R. H. (1981). Principles in his nature which interest him in the fortune of others: The heritability of empathie concern for others. Journal of Personality, 49, 237–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCall, R. B. (1979). Qualitative transitions in behavioral development in the first two years. In M. H. Bornstein & W. Kessen (Eds.), Psychological development in infancy (pp. 182–224). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCord, L. (1990). Altruism, mutual aid and aggression. Paper presentation at workshop, hostility and sociability: Developmental and socio-psychological perspectives, Warsaw, Poland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 324–344.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mills, R. S. L., & Grusec, J. E. (1989). Cognitive, affective, and behavioral consequences of praising altruism. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 299–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muuss, R. E. (1988). Carol Gilligan’s theory of sex differences in the development of moral reasoning during adolescence. Adolescence, 23, 229–243.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelms, B. C. (1989). Emotional behaviors in chronically ill children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 657–668.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholich, L. (1977). Beyond sensorimotor intelligence: Assessment of symbolic maturity through analyses of pretend play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 23, 89–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 257–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olweus, D., Block, J., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1986). Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parke, R. D., & Slaby, R. G. (1983). The development of aggression. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, pp. 547–641). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parmelee, A. (1986). Children’s illnesses: Their beneficial effects on behavioral development. Child Development, 57, 1–10.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pecukis, E. V. (1990). A cognitive/affective empathy training program as a function of ego development in aggressive adolescent females. Adolescence, 25, 59–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgement of the child. New York: Harcourt, Brace. (Originally published 1932).

    Google Scholar 

  • Radke-Yarrow, M., Zahn-Waxier, C., & Chapman, M. (1983). Children’s prosocial dispositions and behavior. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, pp. 469–545). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redefer, L. A., & Goodman, J. F. (1989). Pet-facilitated therapy with autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 461–467.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rehberg, H. R., & Richman, C. L. (1989). Prosocial behavior in preschool children: A look at the interaction of race, gender, and family composition. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 385–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renshaw, P. D., & Asher, S. R. (1983). Children’s goals and strategies for social interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 353–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheingold, H. L., & Emery, G. N. (1986). The nurturant acts of very young children. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), The development of anti-and prosocial behavior (pp. 75–96). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheingold, H. L., Hay, D. F., & West, M. J. (1976). Sharing in the second year of life. Child Development, 47, 1148–1158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robins, L. N. (1986). The consequences of conduct disorder in girls. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior: Research, theories and issues (pp. 385–414). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, J. (1989). Sex differences in the development of empathy during late infancy: Findings from the MacArthur longitudinal twin study. Presentation at SRCD, Kansas City, Missouri.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenhan, D. L. (1970). The natural socialization of altruistic autonomy. In J. R. Macaulay & L. Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior (pp. 251–268). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, H. S., & Goldman, B. D. (1977). Establishing new social relations in infancy. In T. Alloway, L. Kramer, & P. Pliner (Eds.), Advances in the study of communication and affect, Attachment behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 61–79). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rushton, J. P. (1976). Socialization and the altruistic behavior of children, Psychological Bulletin, 83, 898–913.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rushton, J. P., Fulker, D. W., Neale, M. C., Nias, D. L. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Altruism and aggression: The heritability of individual differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1192–1198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing. Evanston IL: Row and Peterson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, N. L. (1984). Cooperation, competition, and altruism within twin sets: A reappraisal. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 163–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, N. L. (1988). Cooperation, competition, and altruism in human twinships: A sociobiological approach. In K. B. MacDonald (Ed.), Sociobiological perspectives on human development (pp. 168–206). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shigetomi, C. C., Hartmann, D. P., & Gelfand, D. M. (1981). Sex differences in children’s altruistic behavior and reputations for helpfulness. Developmental Psychology, 17, 434–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M. (1985). Mother-child relations and the development of empathy: A short-term longitudinal study. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 57, 355–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., Ungerer, J. A., & Russell, A. (1983). Moral judgements in relation to behavioral and cognitive disorders in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, 503–511.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn’s response to the cry of another infant. Developmental Psychology, 5, 136–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanhope, L., Bell, R. Q., & Parker-Cohen, N. Y. (1987). Temperament and helping behavior in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 347–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the “infant”. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strain, P., & Odom, S. L. (1986). Peer social initiations: Effective intervention for social skills development of exceptional children. Exceptional Children, 56, 543–551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (1989). Children’s empathy and role taking: Child and parental factors, and relations to prosocial behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, 227–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Termine, N. T., & Izard, C. E. (1988). Infants’ responses to their mothers’ expressions of joy and sadness. Developmental Psychology, 24, 223–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teti, D. M., & Ablard, K. A. (1989). Security of attachment and infant-sibling relationships: A laboratory study. Child Development, 60, 1519–1528.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1980). Empathy and the development of guilt in children. Developmental Psychology, 16, 155–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1989, Autumn). Origins and directions for the concept of infant intersubjectivity. Society for Research in Child Development. Newsletter, pp. 1-4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, L. J. (1984). Sex differences in the development of moral reasoning: A critical review. Child Development, 55, 677–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, M., & Fischer, K. (1977). A developmental sequence of agent use in late infancy. Child Development, 48, 828–836.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiesenfeld, A. R., Whitman, P B., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1984). Individual differences among adult women in sensitivity to infants: Evidence in support of an empathy concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 118–124.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yarrow, M. R., Scott, P. M., & Waxier, C. Z. (1973). Learning concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 8, 240–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Cole, P., & Barrett, K. C. (1991). Guilt and empathy: Sex differences and implications for the development of depression. In K. Dodge & J. Garber (Eds.), Emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 243–272). New York: Cambridge Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Cummings, E. M., & Iannotti, R. (1986). Altruism and aggression: Biological and social origins. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Friedman, S. L., & Cummings, E. M. (1983). Children’s emotions and behaviors in response to infants’ cries. Child Development, 54, 1522–1528.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., & Kochanska, G. (1990). The development of guilt. In R. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1988: Socioemotional Development (pp. 183–258). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Kochanska, G., Krupnick, J., & McKnew, D. (1990). Patterns of guilt in children of depressed and well mothers. Developmental Psychology, 26(1), 51–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1982). The development of altruism: Alternative research strategies. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 109–137). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. (1979). Child rearing and children’s prosocial initiations toward victims of distress. Child Development, 50, 319–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Chapman, M., & Wagner, E. (1990). The development of concern for others. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Robinson, J., & Emde, R. (1990). The development and heritability of empathy. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zarbatany, L., Hartmann, D. P., & Gelfand, D. M. (1985). Why does children’s generosity increase with age: Susceptibility to experimenter influence or altruism? Child Development, 56, 746–756.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Zahn-Waxler, C., Smith, K.D. (1992). The Development of Prosocial Behavior. In: Van Hasselt, V.B., Hersen, M. (eds) Handbook of Social Development. Perspectives in Developmental Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0694-6_10

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0694-6_10

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-0696-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-0694-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics