Skip to main content

A Retrospective Look at Clinical Pragmatics

  • Chapter
Pragmatics of Language

Abstract

Approximately 20 years ago when I was a graduate student in the psycholinguistics and speech-language pathology programs at the University of Illinois, Tom Shriner, Carol Prutting, and I began our endless discussions about pragmatics, although we didn’t use that term then. We were trying to integrate three modes of thinking about language that existed in three physically distinct locations on campus. One mode of thinking about language was linguistics. In linguistics classes we discussed Chomsky’s theory of generative grammar, a language theory with such profound implications that it was receiving extraordinary attention even in the popular literature. Chomsky’s theory rested on three fundamental assumptions. One was that the domain of linguistic theory was the characterization of the abstract rules of language as language competence rather than language performance. The second assumption was that the primary power of language resided in syntax. The third assumption was that knowledge of syntax could be described independently from the other levels of language knowledge, phonology and semantics. In linguistics classes we analyzed sentences as they “might be written on a blackboard” and we conceptualized language as an abstract, symbolic code. In psychology seminars, brilliant scholars, such as Charles Osgood, taught us about the principles of mediational behavioristic psychology and how these principles were evident in language performance. In these classes we discussed behavior and language. In speech-language pathology, we worked with children and adults who were language disordered. These people were having such difficulty communicating that their functional capabilities were reduced significantly from those of their chronological peer groups. Problems with trying to deal with communication difficulties by viewing language as simply a symbolic code, however elegant, or viewing language as simply another form of behavior were haunting. Although we did not have theoretical models that adequately characterized it as such or a vocabulary that provided a means for making clear references to it, we struggled with trying to deal with communication, language as behavior, with language as it was used by people in their daily lives. This meant that we somehow had to conceptualize language as a code that was used by people to do things. As I review the clinical literature over the last 20 years I think that my experience as a graduate student was to some extent a microcosm of the tensions and struggles that have characterized the study of language disorders itself.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Austin, J. (1962). How to do things with words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1976). Language in context. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Bretherton, I., and Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bedwinek, A. (1983). The use of PACE to facilitate gestural and verbal communication in a language-impaired child. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 14, 2–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blagden, C., & McConnell, N. (1985). Interpersonal language skills assessment. Moline, IL: LinguiSystems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bray, C., and Wiig, E. (1987). Let’s talk inventory for children. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinton, B. (1990). Peer commentary on “Clinical pragmatics: Expectations and realizations” by Tanya Gallagher. Journal of Speech Language Pathology and Audiology, 14 (1), 7–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craig, H. K. (1983). Applications of pragmatic language models for intervention. In T. M. Gallagher and C. A. Prutting (Eds.), Pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language (pp. 101–127 ). San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craig, H. K. (1990). Peer commentary on “Clinical pragmatics: Expectations and realizations” by Tanya Gallagher. Journal of Speech Language Pathology and Audiology, 14 (1), 8–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duchan, J. (1984). Language assessment: The pragmatics revolution. In R. Naremore (Ed.), Language science. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, T. (1983). Pre-assessment: A procedure for accommodating language use variability. In T. Gallagher & C. Prutting (Eds.), Pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language (pp. 1–28 ). San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, T. (1990). Clinical pragmatics: Expectations and realizations. Journal of Speech Language Pathology and Audiology, 14 (1), 3–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lund, N., and Duchan, J. (1988). Assessing children’s language in naturalistic contexts. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLean, J., and Snyder-McLean, L. (1988). Application of pragmatics to severely mentally retarded children and youth. In R. L. Schiefelbusch and L. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspectives: Acquisition, retardation, and intervention ( 2nd ed., pp. 255–288 ). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • McTear, M. (1985). Pragmatic disorders: A question of direction. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 20, 119–127.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Muma, J. (1975). The communication game: Dump and play. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 40, 296–309.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Olswang, L., Kriegsmann, E., and Mastergeorge, A. (1982). Facilitating functional requesting in pragmatically impaired children. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 16, 202–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owens, R. (1991). Language disorders: A functional approach to assessment and intervention. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penn, C. (1988). The profiling of syntax and pragmatics in aphasia. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 2, 179–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prutting, C., and Kirchner, D. (1983). Applied pragmatics. In T. Gallagher and C. Prutting (Eds.), Pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language (pp. 29–64 ). San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prutting, C., and Kirchner, D. (1987). A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 105–119.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rees, N. (1978). Pragmatics of language: Applications to normal and disordered language development. In R. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Bases of language intervention. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rees, N. (Speaker) (1988). Pragmatics: A retrospective analysis (Cassette Recording H81118–110 ). Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, F., and Spekman, N. (1984). Assessing the pragmatic abilities of children: Part I. Organizational framework and assessment parameters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49, 2–11.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, B. (1986). Test of Pragmatic Skills—Revised. Tuscon, AZ: Communication Skill Builders.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skarakis-Doyle, E. (1990). Peer commentary on “Clinical pragmatics: Expectations and realizations” by Tanya Gallagher. Journal of Speech Language Pathology and Audiology, 14 (1), 7–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanRiper, C. (1939). Speech correction: Principles and methods. New York: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B. (1977a). Development of functional communication competencies pre-K through grade 6. Urbana, IL: Eric Clearinghouse in Reading Communication Skills.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B. (1977b). Development of functional communication competencies grade 7–12. Urbana, IL: Eric Clearinghouse in Reading Communication Skills.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1991 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gallagher, T.M. (1991). A Retrospective Look at Clinical Pragmatics. In: Gallagher, T.M. (eds) Pragmatics of Language. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-7156-2_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-7156-2_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-0-412-46850-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-7156-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics