Skip to main content

Research Methods for Studying Daily Life: Experience Sampling and a Multilevel Approach to Study Time and Mood at Work

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Temporal Structure of Multimodal Communication

Part of the book series: Intelligent Systems Reference Library ((ISRL,volume 164))

  • 459 Accesses

Abstract

The Experience Sampling Method (ESM) allows the examination of ongoing thoughts, feelings and actions as they occur in the course of everyday life. A prime benefit is that it captures events in their natural context, thereby complementing information obtained by more traditional techniques. We used ESM to study time and mood at work. Our data were collected by sending 30 text messages over 10 working days to each of 168 part-time workers. On each occasion, respondents assessed their mood. We explored the joint effects of three sets of variables: activities in which people are engaged; individual differences; and time (i.e., when mood is measured). Since the data in our study can be thought of as being collected at two levels, we applied techniques of hierarchical linear models. The results indicated that activities were significant but no systematic individual differences were detected. There were some small diurnal effects as well as an overall “Friday effect.” Lastly, the weather had little or no influence on self-reported mood state. We discuss the results in terms of their methodological implications for studying daily life.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Seasonal effects of weather on moods and behavior have been documented (see, e.g., Smith 1979; Harmatz 2000).

  2. 2.

    The reference category used as a base for coding the dummy variables for different activities was “Housework, personal time organization, and managing funds.”

  3. 3.

    We calculated z-scores for each individual respondent such that the mean of each person’s mood judgments was 0 with a standard deviation of 1. This allowed us to categorize all observations/occasions as being positive or negative, i.e., whether they were above or below each individual’s mean mood score.

  4. 4.

    These results are robust to analyses assuming fixed or random coefficients.

  5. 5.

    These included: daily average temperature (C); precipitation (liters per square meter); rain (dummy variable, 1:yes; 0:not); daily sunshine (total number of hours); relative daily sunshine (percentage out of expected total hours); degree of cloudy at 7am (scale from 0 to 8); degree of cloudy at 1pm (scale from 0 to 8); daily solar radiation (watts per square meter); daily average of relative humidity (%); and daily average of barometric pressure (in hectoPascals, hPa). The data were obtained from the Servei Meteorolgic de Catalunya, Xarxa d’Estacions Meteorolgiques Automtiques (XEMA) del Valls Occidental and Observatori Fabra (Barcelona).

  6. 6.

    We use the term “reactivity” to mean a phenomenon that occurs when individuals alter their performance or behavior due to the awareness that they are being observed.

References

  • Anguera MT (2003) Observational methods (general). In: Fernández-Ballesteros R (ed) Encyclopedia of behavioral assessment, vol 2. Sage. London, UK, pp 632–637

    Google Scholar 

  • Anguera MT, Camerino O, Castañer M, Sánchez-Algarra P, Onwuegbuzie AJ (2017) The specificity of observational studies in physical activity and sports sciences: moving forward in mixed methods research and proposals for achieving quantitative and qualitative symmetry. Front Psychol 8:2196. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02196

  • Bakker AB, Daniels K (eds) (2013) A day in the life of a happy worker. Psychology Press, Hove Sussex

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell IH, Lim MH, Rossell SL, Thomas N (2017) Ecological momentary assessment and intervention in the treatment of psychotic disorders: A systematic review. Psychiatric services. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.201600523

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair AJ, Leakey PN, Rust SR, Shaw S, Benison D, Sandler DA (1999) Locus of control and mood following myocardial infarction. Coron Health Care 3:140–144

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolger N, De Longis A, Kessler RC, Schilling EA (1989) Effects of daily stress on negative mood. J Pers Soc Psychol 57(5):808–818

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradburn NM (1969) The structure of psychological well-being. Aldine, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley MM, Lang PJ (1994) Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 25:49–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunswik E (1944) Distal focusing of perception: Size constancy in a representative sample of situations. Psychol Monogr 56:1–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunswik E (1956) Perception and the representative design of experiments, 2nd edn. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrk AS, Raudenbush SW (2002) Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods, 2nd edn. Sage Publications, Newbury Park

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark LA, Watson D (1988) Mood and the mundane: Relations between daily life events and self-reported mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54(2):296–308

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark LA, Watson D, Leeka J (1988) Diurnal variation in the positive affects. Motiv Emot 13(3):205–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cojuharenco I, Ryvkin D (2008) Peak-end rule versus average utility: How utility aggregation affects evaluation of experiences. J Math Psychol 52:326–335

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Conner TS, Mehl MR (2015) Ambulatory assessment methods for studying everyday life. In: Scott R, Kosslyn S, Pinkerton N (eds) Emerging trends in the social and behavioral sciences, Wiley, Hoboken

    Google Scholar 

  • Conner TS, Tennen H, Fleeson W, Barrett LF (2009) Experience sampling methods: A modern idiographic approach to personality research. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 3(3):292–313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cordier R, Brown N, Chen YW, Wilkes-Gillan S, Falkmer T (2014) Piloting the use of experience sampling method to investigate the everyday social experiences of children with asperger syndrome/high functioning autism. Dev Neurorehabilitation 19(2):103–110

    Google Scholar 

  • Creswell JW, Plano Clark VL (2011) Designing and conducting mixed methods research, 2nd edn. Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi M (1990) Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. HarperCollins, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham MR (1979) Weather, mood, and helping behavior: Quasi experiments with the sunshine samaratin. J Pers Soc Psychol 37(11):1947–1956

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • David JP, Green PJ, Martin R, Suls J (1997) Differential roles of neuroticism, extraversion, and event desirability for mood in daily life: An integrative model of top-down and bottom-up influences. J Pers Soc Psychol 73(1):149–159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denissen JJA, Butalid L, Penke L, van Aken MAG (2008) The effects of weather on daily mood: A multilevel approach. Emotion 8(5):662–667

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener E, Seligman MEP (2004) Beyond money: Towards an economy of well-being. Psychol Sci Public Interes 5(1):1–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener E, Oishi S, Lucas RE (2003) Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Ann Rev Psychol 54:403–425

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doherty ST, Lemieux CJ, Canally C (2014) Tracking human activity and well-being in natural environments using wearable sensors and experience sampling. Soc Sci Med 106:83–92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubad M, Winsper C, Meyer C, Livanou M, Marwaha S (2017) A systematic review of the psychometric properties, usability and clinical impacts of mobile mood-monitoring applications in young people. Psychol Med. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001659

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fahrenberg J, Myrtek M, Pawlik K, Perrez M (2007) Ambulatory assessment monitoring behavior in daily life settings: A behavioral-scientific challenge for psychology. Eur J Psychol Assess 23(4):206–213

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederickson B, Kahneman D (1993) Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. J Pers Soc Psychol 65:45–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey BS, Stutzer A (2002) What can economists learn from happiness research? J Econ Lit 40:402–435

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein H (1995) In: Arnold E (ed) Multilevel statistical models, vol 3, 2nd edn. Kendalls library of statistics. London

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein KM (1972) Weather, mood, and internal-external control. Percept Mot Ski 35:786

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmatz MG, Well AD, Overtree CE, Kawamura KY, Rosal M, Ockene IS (2000) Seasonal variation of depression and other moods: A longitudinal approach. J Biol Rhythm 15:344–350

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hektner JM, Schmidt JA, Csikszentmihalyi M (2007) Experience sampling method: Measuring the quality of everyday life. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hirshleifer D, Shumway T (2003) Good day sunshine: Stock returns and the weather. J Financ 58(3):1009–1032

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth RM, Portell M, Cuxart A, Kolev GI (2011) Emotion and reason in everyday risk perception. J Behav Decis Mak 24:202–222. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.689

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth RM (2005) The challenge of representative design in psychology and economics. J Econ Methodol 12:253–263

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth RM, Portell M, Cuxart A (2007) What risks do people perceive in everyday life? A perspective gained from the experience sampling method (ESM). Risk Anal 27(6):1427–1439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howarth E, Hoffman MS (1984) A multidimensional approach to the relationship between mood and weather. Br J Psychol 75:15–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D, Krueger AB, Schkade DA, Schwarz N, Stone AA (2004) A survey method of characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science 306(5702):1776–1780

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller MC, Fredrickson BL, Ybarra O, Cté S, Johnson K, Mikels J, Conway A, Wager T (2005) A warm heart and a clear head: The contingent effects of weather in mood and cognition. Psychol Sci 16(9):724–731

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemp S, Burt CDB, Furneaux L (2008) A test of the peak-end rule with extended autobiographical events. Mem Cogn 36(1):132–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klonowicz T (2001) Discontented people: Reactivity and locus of control as determinants of subjective well-being. Eur J Pers 15:29–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen RJ, Fredrickson BL (1999) Measurement issues in emotion research. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N (eds) Well-being: foundations of hedonic psychology. Russell Sage, New York, pp 40–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen RJ, Kasimatis M (1990) Individual differences in entrainment of mood to the weekly calendar. J Pers Soc Psychol 58(1):164–171

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liao Y, Skelton K, Dunton G, Bruening M (2016) A systematic review of methods and procedures used in ecological momentary assessments of diet and physical activity research in youth: An adapted strobe checklist for reporting ema studies (cremas). J Med Internet Res 18(6):151 https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.4954

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Longford NT (1993) Random coefficient models. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Magnusson M, Burgoon J, Casarrubea M (eds) (2016) Discovering hidden temporal patterns in behavior interaction. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehl MR, Conner TS (2012) Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. The Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Monk T, Fookson J, Moline M, Pollak C (1985) Diurnal variation in mood and performance in a time-isolated environment. Chronobiol Int 2(3):185–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz DS, Russell JJ, Sadikaj G, Sutton R (2009) Measuring people intensively. Can Psychol 50(3):131–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray G, Allen NB, Trinder J (2002) Mood and the circadian system: Investigation of a circadian component in positive affect. Chronobiol Int 19(6):1151–1169

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray G, Nicholas CL, Kleiman J, Dwyer R, Carrington MJ, Allen NB, Trinder J (2009) Nature’s clocks and human mood: The circadian system modulates reward motivation. Emotion 9(5):705–716

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oishi S, Diener E, Choi DW, Kim-Prieto C, Choi I (2007) The dynamics of daily events and well-being across cultures: When less is more. J Pers Soc Psychol 93(4):685–698

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Os J, Verhagen S, Marsman A, Peeters F, Bak M, Marcelis M, Glksz S (2017) The experience sampling method as an mhealth tool to support self-monitoring, self-insight, and personalized health care in clinical practice. Depress Anxiety 34(6):481–493

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parrott WG, Sabini J (1990) Mood and memory under natural conditions: Evidence for mood incongruent recall. J Pers Soc Psychol 59(2):321–336

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peeters F, Berkhof J, Delespaul P, Rottenberg J, Nicolson NA (2006) Diurnal mood variation in major depressive disorder. Emotion 6(3):383–391

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Portell M, Anguera MT, Chacón-Moscoso S, Sanduvete-Chaves S (2015a) Guidelines for reporting evaluations based on observational methodology. Psicothema 27(3):283–289

    Google Scholar 

  • Portell M, Anguera MT, Hernández-Mendo A, Jonsson GK (2015c) Quantifying biopsychosocial aspects in everyday contexts: an integrative methodological approach from the behavioral sciences. Psychol Res Behav Manag 8:153–160

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Portell M, Anguera MT, Hernández-Mendo A, Jonsson GK (2015b) The legacy of brunswik’s representative design in the 21st century: methodological innovations for studying everyday life. The Brunswik Society Newsletter. http://www.brunswik.org/newsletters/2015news.pdf. Accessed Dec 2015

  • Rind B (1966) Effects of beliefs about weather conditions on tipping. J Appl Soc Psychol 26(2):137–147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rind B, Strohmetz D (2001) Effects of belief about future weather conditions on restaurant tipping. J Appl Soc Psychol 31(10):2160–2164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossi AS, Rossi PE (1977) Body time and social time: Mood patterns by menstrual cycle phase and day of the week. Soc Sci Res 6:273–308

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rotter JB (1966) Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol Monogr 80(1, Whole No. 609)

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Ruys KI, Stapel DA (2008) The secret life of emotions. Psychol Sci 19:385–391

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders JL, Brizzolara MS (1982) Relationships between weather and mood. J Gen Psychol 107:155–156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandstrom GM, Lathia N, Mascolo C, Rentfrow PJ (2017) Putting mood in context: Using smartphones to examine how people feel in different locations. J Res Pers 69:96–101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santangelo P, Bohus M, Ebner-Priemer UW (2014) Ecological momentary assessment in borderline personality disorder: A review of recent findings and methodological challenges. J Pers Disord 28(4):555–576

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders EM (1993) Stock prices and wall street weather. Am Econ Rev 83:1337–1345

    Google Scholar 

  • Schkade DA, Kahneman D (1998) Does living in california make people happy? A Focus Illusion JudgmS Life Satisf 9(5):340–346

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz N, Clore GL (1983) Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. J Pers Soc Psychol 45(3):513–523

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR (2008) Ecological momentary assessment. Ann Rev Clin Psychol 4:1–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvia PJ, Kwapil TR, Walsh MA, Myin-Germeys I (2014) Planned missing data designs in experience sampling research: Monte carlo simulations of efficient designs for assessing within-person constructs. Behav Res Methods 46(1):41–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonsohn U (2007) Clouds make nerds look good: Field evidence of the impact of incidental factors on decision making. J Behav Decis Mak 20(2):143–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith TW (1979) Happiness: Time trends, seasonal variations, intersurvey differences, and other mysteries. Soc Psychol Q 42(1):18–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack F, Argyle M, Schwartz N (eds) (1991) Subjective wellbeing: An interdisciplinary perspective. Pergamon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone AA, Hedges SM, Neale JM, Satin MS (1985) Prospective and cross-sectional mood reports offer no evidence of a blue monday phenomenon. J Pers Soc Psychol 49(1):129–134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone AA, Smyth JM, Pickering T, Schwartz J (1996) Daily mood variability: Form of diurnal patterns and determinants of diurnal patterns. J Appl Soc Psychol 26(14):1286–1305

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone AA, Schwartz JE, Schwartz N, Schkade D, Krueger A, Kahneman D (2006) A population approach to the study of emotion: Diurnal rhythms of a working day examined with the day reconstruction method. Emotion 6(1):139–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack F, Martin LL, Schwartz N (1988) Priming and communication; social determinants of information use in judgments of life satisfaction. Eur J Soc Psychol 18(5):429–442

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thayer RE (1987) Problem perception, optimism, and related states as a function of the time of day (diurnal rhythm) and moderate exercise: Two arousal systems in interaction. Motiv Emot 11(1):19–36

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Trombley MA (1997) Stock prices and wall street weather: Additional evidence. Q J Bus Econ 36:11–21

    Google Scholar 

  • Trull TJ, Ebner-Priemer U (2014) The role of ambulatory assessment in psychological science. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 23(6):466–470

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson D, Tellegen A (1985) Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychol Bull 98:219–235

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson D, Wiese D, Vaidya J, Tellegen A (1999) The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol 76(5):820–838

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wen CKF, Schneider S, Stone AA, Spruijt-Metz D (2017) Compliance with mobile ecological momentary assessment protocols in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Med Internet Res 19(4):132. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.6641

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood C, Magnello M (1992) Diurnal changes in perceptions of energy and mood. J R Soc Med 85:191–194

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood C, Magnello M, Sharpe MC (1992) Fluctuations in perceived energy and mood among patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. J R Soc Med 85:195–198

    Google Scholar 

  • Volmer J, Fritsche A (2016) Daily negative work events and employees’ physiological and psychological reactions. Front Psychol 7:1711. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01711

  • Zembylas M, Schutz PA (eds) (2016) Methodological advances in research on emotion and education. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was partially supported by Spanish Government grant [SEJ2006-27587-E/SOCI, DEP2015- 66069-P, and PSI2015-71947-REDT], as well as was partially supported by Generalitat de Catalunya [2014 SGR 971].

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mariona Portell .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Portell, M., Hogarth, R.M., Cuxart, A. (2020). Research Methods for Studying Daily Life: Experience Sampling and a Multilevel Approach to Study Time and Mood at Work. In: Hunyadi, L., Szekrényes, I. (eds) The Temporal Structure of Multimodal Communication. Intelligent Systems Reference Library, vol 164. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-22895-8_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics