Skip to main content

Self-Organisation of Conceptual Spaces from Quality Dimensions

  • Chapter
Applications of Conceptual Spaces

Part of the book series: Synthese Library ((SYLI,volume 359))

Abstract

This chapter presents a discussion on how conceptual spaces can evolve from a set of quality dimensions, and how these spaces can become shared among a population of cognitive agents. An agent-based simulation of Steels’ Talking Heads experiment is presented in which virtual agents construct novel concepts, as well as a shared, simplified language from scratch. Simulations demonstrate that the structure of a conceptual space (i.e. from what quality dimensions it is composed) can evolve in a population of communicating agents. It is argued that the underlying mechanisms involve the following factors: the environment of the agents, their embodiment and cognitive capacities, self-organisation, and cultural transmission.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Arbib, M. A., & Bickerton, D. (Eds.). (2010). The emergence of protolanguage: Holophrasis vs compositionality. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M. C. (2003). Linguistic differences and language design. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(8), 349–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baronchelli, A., Felici, M., Caglioti, E., Loreto, V., & Steels, L. (2006). Sharp transition towards shared lexicon in multi-agent systems. Journal of Statistical Mechanics, P06014.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batali, J. (2002). The negotiation and acquisition of recursive grammars as a result of competition among exemplars. In T. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic evolution through language acquisition: Formal and computational models (chap. 5). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 173–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bod, R., Sima’an, K., & Scha, R. (Eds.). (2003). Data oriented parsing. Stanford: Center for Study of Language and Information (CSLI) Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M., & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.). (2001). Language acquisition and conceptual development. Cambridge: Cambrige University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brighton, H., & Kirby, S. (2001). The survival of the smallest: Stability conditions for the cultural evolution of compositional language. In J. Kelemen & P. SosĂ­k (Eds.), Proceeding of the 6th European conference on artificial life, ECAL 2001, Prague.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croft, W. (2000). Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croft, W. (2002). The Darwinization of linguistics. Selection, 3, 75–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1968). The origin of species. Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Beule, J., & Bergen, B. K. (2006). On the emergence of compositionality. In A. Cangelosi, A. Smith, & K. Smith (Eds.), The evolution of language: Proceedings of the 6th international conference on the evolution of language, Rome.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Boer, B., & Vogt, P. (1999). Emergence of speech sounds in changing populations. In D. Floreano, J.-D. Nicoud, & F. Mondada (Eds.), Advances in artificial life: Proceedings of 5th European conference ECAL’99, Lausanne.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Greeff, J., & Belpaeme, T. (2011). The development of shared meaning within different embodiments. In 2011 IEEE international conference on development and learning (ICDL), Frankfurt am Main (Vol. 2, pp. 1–6).

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(05), 429–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (2000). Conceptual spaces. Cambridge: Bradford Books/MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrod, S., Fay, N., Rogers, S., Walker, B., & Swoboda, N. (2010). Can iterated learning explain the emergence of graphical symbols? Interaction Studies, 11(1), 33–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haun, D., Rapold, C. J., Janzen, G., & Levinson, S. C. (2011). Plasticity of human spatial cognition: Spatial language and cognition covary across cultures. Cognition, 119(1), 70–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of language. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kalish, M. L., Griffiths, T. L., & Lewandowsky, S. (2007). Iterated learning: Intergenerational knowledge transmission reveals inductive biases. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(2), 288–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S. (2001). Spontaneous evolution of linguistic structure: An iterated learning model of the emergence of regularity and irregularity. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation, 5(2), 102–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S., & Hurford, J. R. (2002). The emergence of linguistic structure: An overview of the iterated learning model. In A. Cangelosi & D. Parisi (Eds.), Simulating the evolution of language (pp. 121–148). London: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S., Smith, K., & Brighton, H. (2004). From UG to universals: Linguistic adaptation through iterated learning. Studies in Language, 28(3), 587–607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(31), 10681–10686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koolen, R., Gatt, A., Goudbeek, M., & Krahmer, E. (2011). Factors causing overspecification in definite descriptions. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(13), 3231–3250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D., & Levinson, S. C. (2004). Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 108–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mufwene, S. S. (2001). The ecology of language evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selecion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prigogine, I., & Strengers, I. (1984). Order out of chaos. New York: Bantam Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L. (1996). Perceptually grounded meaning creation. In M. Tokoro (Ed.), Proceedings of the international conference on multi-agent systems, Kyoto. Menlo Park: AAAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L. (1997). Synthesising the origins of language and meaning using coevolution, self-organisation and level formation. In J. Hurford, C. Knight, & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L. (2003). Evolving grounded communication for robots. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 308–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L. (Ed.). (2012). Experiments in cultural language evolution (Vol. 3). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L., & Belpaeme, T. (2005). Coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: A case study for colour. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 469–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L., & Beule, J. D. (2006). Unify and merge in fluid construction grammar. In P. Vogt, Y. Sugita, E. Tuci, & C. Nehaniv (Eds.), Symbol grounding and beyond: Proceedings of the third international workshop on the emergence and evolution of linguistic communication, Rome (pp. 197–223). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L., & Kaplan, F. (1998). Stochasticity as a source of innovation in language games. In Proceedings of alive VI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steels, L., Kaplan, F., McIntyre, A., & Van Looveren, J. (2002). Crucial factors in the origins of word-meaning. In A. Wray (Ed.), The transition to language (pp. 252–271). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Zaanen, M. (2000). ABL: Alignment-based learning. In Proceedings of the 18th international conference on computational linguistics (COLING), SaarbrĂĽcken.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, P. (2004). Minimum cost and the emergence of the Zipf-Mandelbrot law. In J. Pollack, M. Bedau, P. Husbands, T. Ikegami, & R. A. Watson (Eds.), Artificial life IX proceedings of the ninth international conference on the simulation and synthesis of living systems, Boston (pp. 214–219). MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, P. (2005a). The emergence of compositional structures in perceptually grounded language games. Artificial Intelligence, 167(1–2), 206–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, P. (2005b). Meaning development versus predefined meanings in language evolution models. In L. Kaelbling & A. Saffiotti (Eds.), Proceedings of IJCAI-05, Edinburgh (pp. 1154–1159). IJCAI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, P. (2005c). On the acquisition and evolution of compositional languages: Sparse input and the productive creativity of children. Adaptive Behavior, 13(4), 325–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, P. (2006a). Cumulative cultural evolution: Can we ever learn more? In S. Nolfi et al. (Eds.), From animals to animats 9: Proceedings of the ninth international conference on simulation of adaptive behaviour, Rome. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, P. (2006b). Overextensions and the emergence of compositionality. In A. Cangelosi, A. Smith, & K. Smith (Eds.), The evolution of language: Proceedings of the 6th international conference on the evolution of language, Rome. World Scientific Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, P. (2007). Variation, competition and selection in the self-organisation of compositionality. In B. Wallace, A. Ross, J. B. Davies, & T. Anderson (Eds.), The mind, the body and the world: Psychology after cognitivism? (pp. 233–256). Exeter: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, P., & Mastin, J. D. (2013). Anchoring social symbol grounding in children’s interactions. KĂĽnstliche Intelligenz, 27, 145–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellens, P., Loetzsch, M., & Steels, L. (2008). Flexible word meaning in embodied agents. Connection Science, 20(2), 173–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wray, A. (1998). Protolanguage as a holistic systemfor social interaction. Language and Communication, 18, 47–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The writing of this chapter was funded through a Vidi grant awarded by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO, grant no. 276-70-018). I wish to thank Frank Zenker, Peter Gärdenfors and all participants of the Conceptual Spaces at Work symposium for their valuable contributions in discussing this research. Also, many thanks to Emiel Krahmer and an anonymous reviewer for their valuable comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Vogt .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Vogt, P. (2015). Self-Organisation of Conceptual Spaces from Quality Dimensions. In: Zenker, F., Gärdenfors, P. (eds) Applications of Conceptual Spaces. Synthese Library, vol 359. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-15021-5_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics