Skip to main content

Volition und kognitive Kontrolle

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Allgemeine Psychologie

Zusammenfassung

Die Forschung zur Volition und kognitiven Kontrolle befasst sich mit den kognitiven und neuronalen Mechanismen, die der willentlichen Steuerung zielgerichteter Handlungen zugrunde liegen. Der Fokus liegt dabei auf Situationen, in denen automatisierte Routinehandlungen nicht ausreichen, sondern Verhaltensdispositionen und kognitive Prozesse auf neue Weise konfiguriert oder konkurrierende Motivationstendenzen und Gewohnheiten unterdrückt werden müssen, um langfristige oder übergeordnete Ziele zu erreichen. Das Kapitel gibt einen Überblick über ausgewählte Theorien und Ergebnisse dieser Forschung anhand von drei Leitfragen: (1) Was sind die kognitiven Grundlagen willentlicher Handlungen? (2) Welche Mechanismen liegen der Fähigkeit zugrunde, Absichten gegen konkurrierende Motivationstendenzen oder Gewohnheiten abzuschirmen? (3) Welche neuronalen Systeme liegen kognitiven Kontrollprozessen und selbstkontrolliertem Verhalten zugrunde?

Schlüsselwörter: Volition; Kognitive Kontrolle; Exekutive Funktionen; Willenshandlungen; Selbstkontrolle; Zielgerichtete Handlungen; Intentionen; Präfrontaler Cortex; Wille

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Literatur

  • Ach, N. (1910). Über den Willensakt und das Temperament. Leipzig: Quelle & Meyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens. In E. Abderhalden (Hrsg.), Handbuch der biologischen Arbeitsmethoden Bd. 6 Berlin: Urban & Schwarzenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Motivation und Volition im Handlungsverlauf. In J. Heckhausen, & H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Motivation und Handeln (4. Aufl. S. 277–300). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Implementation intentions and shielding goal striving from unwanted thoughts and feelings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(3), 381–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ainslie, G. (2005). Precis of Breakdown of Will. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 635–673.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, W. H., & Brown, J. W. (2010). Computational models of performance monitoring and cognitive control. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), 658–677.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta, & M. Moscovitch (Hrsg.), Attention and performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (S. 421–452). Cambridge, MA, USA: Mit Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ansorge, U., & Neumann, O. (2005). Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrast-masked primes: evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cuing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(4), 762–777.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 410–422.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(4), 177–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Azuar, C., Reyes, P., Slachevsky, A., Volle, E., Kinkingnehun, S., Kouneiher, F., & Levy, R. (2014). Testing the model of caudo-rostral organization of cognitive control in the human with frontal lesions. Neuroimage, 84, 1053–1060.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badre, D., & D’Esposito, M. (2007). Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(12), 2082–2099.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badre, D., & D’Esposito, M. (2009). Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(9), 659–669.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Badre, D., Hoffman, J., Cooney, J. W., & D’Esposito, M. (2009). Hierarchical cognitive control deficits following damage to the human frontal lobe. Nature Neuroscience, 12(4), 515–522.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Banks, B. S., & Isham, E. A. (2009). We infer rather than perceive the moment we decide to act. Psychological Science, 20(1), 17–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Striving for unwanted goals: stress-dependent discrepancies between explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective well-being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(5), 781–799.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Self-infiltration; Confusing assigned tasks as self-selected in memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(4), 487–497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(18), 6993–7000.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, S. M., Ruge, H., Walser, M., & Goschke, T. (2014). The functional neuroanatomy of spontaneous retrieval and strategic monitoring of delayed intentions. Neuropsychologia, 52, 37–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergen, A. v (1968). Task interruption. Amsterdam: North Holland Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berking, M., & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion regulation and mental health: recent findings, current challenges, and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25(2), 128–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berkman, E. T., Falk, E. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2011). In the trenches of real-world self-control: neural correlates of breaking the link between craving and smoking. Psychological Science, 22(4), 498–506.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berkman, E. T., Kahn, L. E., & Merchant, J. S. (2014). Training-induced changes in inhibitory control network activity. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(1), 149–157.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C. (2012). From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 1124–1143.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Blunt, A. P. (1998). Volitional action and inaction in the lives of undergraduate students: State orientation, procrastination and proneness to boredom. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 837–846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition: Effects of positive and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological Science, 14(5), 416–421.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boorman, E. D., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2009). How green is the grass on the other side? Frontopolar cortex and the evidence in favor of alternative courses of action. Neuron, 62(5), 733–743.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M., & Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: from behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 83–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. D. (2014). The computational and neural basis of cognitive control: charted territory and new frontiers. Cognitive Science, 38(6), 1249–1285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M., Niv, Y., & Barto, A. C. (2009). Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective. Cognition, 113(3), 262–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braem, S., King, J. A., Korb, F. M., Krebs, R. M., Notebaert, W., & Egner, T. (2013). Affective modulation of cognitive control is determined by performance-contingency and mediated by ventromedial prefrontal and cingulate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(43), 16961–16970.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 946–960.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 106–113.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Braver, T. S., & Bongiolatti, S. R. (2002). The role of frontopolar cortex in subgoal processing during working memory. Neuroimage, 15(3), 523–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. W., & Braver, T. S. (2005). Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science, 307(5712), 1118–1121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. W., Reynolds, J. R., & Braver, T. S. (2007). A computational model of fractionated conflict-control mechanisms in task-switching. Cognitive Psychology, 55(1), 37–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruyneel, L., Steenbergen, H. van, Hommel, B., Band, G. P. H., De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2013). Happy but still focused: failures to find evidence for a mood-induced widening of visual attention. Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung, 77(3), 320–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckholtz, J. W., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2012). Psychopathology and the human connectome: toward a transdiagnostic model of risk for mental illness. Neuron, 74(6), 990–1004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bugg, J. M., Scullin, M. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). Strengthening encoding via implementation intention formation increases prospective memory commission errors. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(3), 522–527.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Buhle, J. T., Silvers, J. A., Wager, T. D., Lopez, R., Onyemekwu, C., Kober, H., & Kober, H. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 24(11), 2981–2990.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bühringer, G., Wittchen, H.-U., Gottlebe, K., Kufeld, C., & Goschke, T. (2008). Why people change? The role of cognitive-control processes in the onset and cessation of substance abuse disorders. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17(Suppl1), 4–S15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bühringer, G., Kräplin, A., & Behrendt, S. (2012). Universal characteristics and consequences of the addiction syndrome. In H. J. Shaffer, D. A. LaPlante, & S. E. Nelson (Hrsg.), foundations, influences, and expressions of addiction APA Addiction syndrome handbook, (Bd. 1, S. 291–316). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, P. W., Veitch, E., Lacy Costello, A. de, & Shallice, T. (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia, 38(6), 848–863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, P. W., Gonen-Yaacovi, G., & Volle, E. (2011). Functional neuroimaging studies of prospective memory: what have we learnt so far? Neuropsychologia, 49(8), 2246–2257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calhoun, V. D., Miller, R., Pearlson, G., & Adali, T. (2014). The chronnectome: time-varying connectivity networks as the next frontier in fMRI data discovery. Neuron, 84(2), 262–274.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carlin, D., Bonerba, J., Phipps, M., Alexander, G., Shapiro, M., & Grafman, J. (2000). Planning impairments in frontal lobe dementia and frontal lobe lesion patients. Neuropsychologia, 38, 655–665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science, 280(5364), 747–749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Somerville, L. H., Gotlib, I. H., Ayduk, O., Franklin, N. T., Askren, M. K., & Shoda, Y. (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 108(36), 14998–15003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chao, L. L., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Contribution of human prefrontal cortex to delay performance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 167–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2011). Positive affect versus reward: emotional and motivational influences on cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 279.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2014). Dissociable influences of reward motivation and positive emotion on cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(2), 509–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cisler, J. M., Olatunji, B. O., Feldner, M. T., & Forsyth, J. P. (2010). Emotion Regulation and the Anxiety Disorders: An Integrative Review. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32(1), 68–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97(3), 332–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. L., Bayer, U. C., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). Self-regulatory strategy and executive control: implementation intentions modulate task switching and Simon task performance. Psychological Research, 72(1), 12–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A.-L., Kantner, J., Dixon, R. A., & Lindsay, D. S. (2011). The intention interference effect: the difficulty of ignoring what you intend to do. Experimental Psychology, 58(6), 425–433.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. W., Yeung, N., Freiwald, W. A., & Botvinick, M. (2009). Cingulate cortex: Diverging data from humans and monkeys. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(11), 566–574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. W., Laurent, P., & Stocco, A. (2013a). Rapid instructed task learning: a new window into the human brain’s unique capacity for flexible cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(1), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. W., Reynolds, J. R., Power, J. D., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., & Braver, T. S. (2013b). Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nature Neuroscience, 16(9), 1348–1355.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cools, R., & D’Esposito, M. (2011). Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), 113–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Crockett, M. J., Braams, B. R., Clark, L., Tobler, P. N., Robbins, T. W., & Kalenscher, T. (2013). Restricting temptations: neural mechanisms of precommitment. Neuron, 79(2), 391–401.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Crone, E. A., Wendelken, C., Donohue, S. E., & Bunge, S. A. (2006). Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. Cerebral Cortex, 16(4), 475–486.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Ardenne, K., Eshel, N., Luka, J., Lenartowicz, A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2012). Role of prefrontal cortex and the midbrain dopamine system in working memory updating. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 109(49), 19900–19909.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daw, N. D., O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature, 441(7095), 876–879.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Posner, M. I., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Localization of a neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 5(5), 303–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (2003). Freedom evolves. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Ridder, D. T. D., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: a meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 76–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derrfuss, J., Brass, M., Neumann, J., & Cramon, D. Y. von (2005). Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 22–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R. (2014). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Working Memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 115–142.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • D’Esposito, M., Aquirre, G. K., Zarahn, E. K., Ballard, D., Shin, R. K., & Lease, J. (1998). Functional MRI studies of spatial and non-spatial working memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diefendorff, J. M., Hall, R. J., Lord, R. G., & Strean, M. L. (2000). Action-state orientation: Construct validity of a revised measure and its relationship to work-related variables. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(2), 250–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dockree, P. M., & Ellis, J. A. (2001). Forming and canceling everyday intentions: Implications for prospective remembering. Memory & Cognition, 29(8), 1139–1145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, R. J., & Dayan, P. (2013). Goals and habits in the brain. Neuron, 80(2), 312–325.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Domachowska, I., Heitmann, C., Deutsch, R., Goschke, T., Scherbaum, S., & Bolte, A. (2016). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention: Registered replication report of Gable and Harmon-Jones (2008). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dörner, D. (1987). Denken und Wollen: Ein systemtheoretischer Ansatz. In P. M. G. H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Jenseits des Rubikon. Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (S. 238–250). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Doya, K. (2008). Modulators of decision making. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 410–416.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dreisbach, G., & Fischer, R. (2012). The role of affect and reward in the conflict-triggered adjustment of cognitive control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 342.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 30(2), 343–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druzgal, T. J., & D’Esposito, M. (2003). Dissecting contributions of prefrontal cortex and fusiform face area to face working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(6), 771–784.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (2010). The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14(4), 172–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 475–483.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durstewitz, D., & Seamans, J. K. (2002). The computational role of dopamine D1 receptors in working memory. Neural Networks, 15(4–6), 561–572.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durstewitz, D., & Seamans, J. K. (2008). The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex dopamine function with relevance to catechol-o-methyltransferase genotypes and schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 64(9), 739–749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ebitz, R. B., & Platt, M. L. (2015). Neuronal activity in primate dorsal anterior cingulate cortex signals task conflict and predicts adjustments in pupil-linked arousal. Neuron, 85(3), 628–640.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, T. (2014). Creatures of habit (and control): a multi-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1247.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature Neuroscience, 8(12), 1784–1790.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(1), 229–240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 148–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Etkin, A. (2012). Neurobiology of anxiety: from neural circuits to novel solutions? Depression and Anxiety, 29(5), 355–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Etkin, A., Büchel, C., & Gross, J. J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(11), 693–700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2016). Drug addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 23–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., & Hoormann, J. (1995). Event-related potential correlates of errors in reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology, 44(Supplement), 287–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Figner, B., Knoch, D., Johnson, E. J., Krosch, A. R., Lisanby, S. H., Fehr, E., & Weber, E. U. (2010). Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 13(5), 538–539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R., Plessow, F., Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2015). Individual differences in the context-dependent recruitment of cognitive control: evidence from action versus state orientation. Journal of Personality, 83(5), 575–583.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forman, S. D., Dougherty, G. G., Casey, B. J., Siegle, G. J., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., & Lorensen, E. (2004). Opiate addicts lack error-dependent activation of rostral anterior cingulate. Biological Psychiatry, 55(5), 531–537.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(3), 220–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franken, I. H., Strien, J. W. van, Franzek, E. J., & Wetering, B. J. van de (2007). Error-processing deficits in patients with cocaine dependence. Biological Psychology, 75(1), 45–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frascella, J., Potenza, M. N., Brown, L. L., & Childress, A. R. (2010). Shared brain vulnerabilities open the way for nonsubstance addictions: carving addiction at a new joint? Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1187, 294–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Positive Emotions Broaden and Build. In D. Patricia, & P. Ashby (Hrsg.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Bd. 47, S. 1–53). Burlington: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J. E., & Ellis, J. A. (2003). The intention-superiority effect for naturally occurring activities: The role of intention accessibility in everyday prospective remembering in young and older adults. International Journal of Psychology, 38(4), 215–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fried, I., Mukamel, R., & Kreiman, G. (2011). Internally generated preactivation of single neurons in human medial frontal cortex predicts volition. Neuron, 69(3), 548–562.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 201–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, J., & Dreisbach, G. (2013). Conflicts as aversive signals: conflict priming increases negative judgments for neutral stimuli. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(2), 311–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frober, K., & Dreisbach, G. (2014). The differential influences of positive affect, random reward, and performance-contingent reward on cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(2), 530–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, K., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Mindsets and pre-conscious open-mindedness to incidental information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1), 48–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 61(2), 331–349.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. M., & Alexander, G. E. (1971). Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science, 173, 652–654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Price, T. F. (2012). The influence of affective states varying in motivational intensity on cognitive scope. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gawrilow, C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). Implementation intentions facilitate response inhibition in children with ADHD. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(2), 261–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaley, A., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 129–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaley, A., Cooney, J., McEvoy, J., Knight, R. T., & d’Esposito, M. (2005). Top-down Enhancement and Suppression of the Magnitude and Speed of Neural Activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(3), 507–517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gell, L., Bühringer, G., McLeod, J., Forberger, S., Holmes, J., Lingford-Hughes, A., & Meier, P. S. (Hrsg.). (2016). What determines harm from addictive substance and behaviours? Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glannon, W. (Hrsg.). (2015). Free Will and the Brain: Neuroscientific, Philosophical, and Legal Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gläscher, J., Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Bechara, A., Rudrauf, D., Calamia, M., & Tranel, D. (2012). Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 109(36), 14681–14686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(11), 652–669.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). Mindset theory of action phases. In P. van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Hrsg.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Bd. 1, S. 526–545). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 186–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets on illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(4), 531–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. In M. P. Zanna (Hrsg.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Bd. 38, S. 69–119). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Steller, B. (1990). Deliberative and implemental mind-sets: Cognitive tuning toward congruous thoughts and information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 1119–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (1996). Wille und Kognition. Zur funktionalen Architektur der intentionalen Handlungssteuerung. In J. Kuhl, & H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Motivation, Volition und Handeln Enzyklopädie der Psychologie Serie IV, (Bd. 4, S. 583–663). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2000). Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task set switching. In S. Monsell, & J. Driver (Hrsg.), Attention and Performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (S. 331–355). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2003). Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. In S. Maasen, W. Prinz, & G. Roth (Hrsg.), Voluntary action: Brains, minds, and sociality (S. 49–85). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2006). Der bedingte Wille: Willensfreiheit aus der Sicht der kognitiven Neurowissenschaft. In G. Roth, & K.-J. Grün (Hrsg.), Das Gehirn und seine Freiheit. Beiträge zur neurowissenschaftlichen Grundlegung der Philosophie (S. 107–156). Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2013). Volition in action: Intentions, control dilemmas and the dynamic regulation of cognitive control. In W. Prinz, A. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Hrsg.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (S. 409–434). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T. (2014). Dysfunctions of decision-making and cognitive control as transdiagnostic mechanisms of mental disorders: advances, gaps, and needs in current research. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 23(S1), 41–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T., & Bolte, A. (2014). Emotional modulation of control dilemmas: the role of positive affect, reward, and dopamine in cognitive stability and flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 62, 403–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T., & Dreisbach, G. (2008). Conflict-triggered goal shielding: Response conflicts attenuate background monitoring for prospective memory cues. Psychological Science, 19(1), 25–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 1211–1226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1996). Remembering what to do: Explicit and implicit memory for intentions. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Hrsg.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (S. 53–91). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T., & Walter, H. (2005). Bewusstsein und Willensfreiheit: Philosophische und empirische Annäherungen. In C. Herrmann, M. Pauen, J. Rieger, & S. Schicktanz (Hrsg.), Bewusstsein: Philosophie, Neurowissenschaften, Ethik (S. 81–119). München: Wilhelm Fink/UTB.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T., Linser, K., & Wendt-Kürschner, J. (2008). Wille und Bewusstsein. In T. Vierkant (Hrsg.), Willenshandlungen: Zur Natur und Kultur der Selbststeuerung (S. 40–66). Frankfurt a.M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 285–312.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(4), 480–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J., & Cohen, J. (2004). For the law, neuroscience changes nothing and everything. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 359(1451), 1775–1785.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J. (2013). Emotion regulation: taking stock and moving forward. Emotion, 13(3), 359–365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, O., Diekhof, E. K., Kirchenbauer, L., & Goschke, T. (2010). A neural system for evaluating the behavioural relevance of salient events outside the current focus of attention. Brain Research, 1351, 212–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, O., Karch, S., Schlueter, E. K., Falkai, P., & Goschke, T. (2006). Neural mechanisms of advance preparation in task switching. Neuroimage, 31(2), 887–895.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gundlach, H. (1987). Anfänge der experimentellen Willenspsychologie. In H. Heckhausen, P. M. Gollwitzer, & F. E. Weinert (Hrsg.), Jenseits des Rubikon. Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (S. 67–85). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, W. (2014). Allgemeine Arbeitspsychologie (3. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haggard, P., & Eimer, M. (1999). On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements. Experimental Brain Research, 126(1), 128–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324(5927), 646–648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, T. A., Malmaud, J., & Rangel, A. (2011). Focusing attention on the health aspects of foods changes value signals in vmPFC and improves dietary choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(30), 11077–11087.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Price, T. F. (2012). The influence of affective states varying in motivational intensity on cognitive scope. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, S. A., & Tong, F. (2009). Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature, 458(7238), 632–635.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hassin, R. R., Ochsner, K., & Trope, Y. (Hrsg.). (2010). Self control in society, mind, and brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, J.-D. (2015). A primer on pattern-based approaches to fMRI: Principles, pitfalls, and perspectives. Neuron, 87(2), 257–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, J.-D., Sakai, K., Rees, G., Gilbert, S., Frith, C., & Passingham, R. E. (2007). Reading Hidden Intentions in the Human Brain. Current Biology, 17(4), 323–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hazy, T. E., Frank, M. J., & O’Reilly, R. C. (2007). Towards an executive without a homunculus: computational models of the prefrontal cortex/basal ganglia system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1485), 1601–1613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heatherton, T. F., & Wagner, D. D. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 132–139.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1989). Motivation und Handeln (2. Aufl.). Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Herd, S. A., O’Reilly, R. C., Hazy, T. E., Chatham, C. H., Brant, A. M., & Friedman, N. P. (2014). A neural network model of individual differences in task switching abilities. Neuropsychologia, 62, 375–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hester, R., Nestor, L., & Garavan, H. (2009). Impaired error awareness and anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivity in chronic cannabis users. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34(11), 2450–2458.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Heuvel, O. A. v.d., Groenewegen, H. J., Barkhof, F., Lazeron, R. H. C., Dyck, R. van, & Veltmana, D. J. (2003). Frontostriatal system in planning complexity: a parametric functional magnetic resonance version of Tower of London task. Neuroimage, 18, 367–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hiemisch, A., Ehlers, A., & Westermann, R. (2002). Mindsets in social anxiety: A new look at selective information processing. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 103–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, J. (1993). Vorhersage und Erkenntnis. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., & Kotabe, H. (2012). A General Model of Preventive and Interventive Self-Control. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(10), 707–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 962–977.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Roefs, A. (2009). Three ways to resist temptation: The independent contributions of executive attention, inhibitory control, and affect regulation to the impulse control of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(2), 431–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Everyday temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflicts, and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1318–1335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holst, R. J. van, Brink, W. van den, Veltman, D. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2010). Why gamblers fail to win: A review of cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(1), 87–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell, & J. Driver (Hrsg.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and Performance XVIII (S. 247–273). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68(1), 1–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huettel, S. A., Song, A. W., & McCarthy, G. (2014). Functional magnetic resonance imaging. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huntsinger, J. R. (2012). Does positive affect broaden and negative affect narrow attentional scope? A new answer to an old question. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 595–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inwagen, P. (1983). An essay on free will. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inzlicht, M., Legault, L., & Teper, R. (2014). Exploring the Mechanisms of Self-Control Improvement. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(4), 302–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (2007). Positive affect, cognitive flexibility, and self-control. In Y. Shoda, D. Cervone, & G. Downey (Hrsg.), Persons in context: Building a science of the individual (S. 130–147). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, J., Heller, K., & Egner, T. (2014). Bayesian modeling of flexible cognitive control. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 1(46), 30–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jostmann, N. B., & Koole, S. L. (2007). On the regulation of cognitive control: Action orientation moderates the impact of high demands in Stroop interference tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(4), 593–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kable, J. W. (2014). Valuation, intertemporal choice, and self-control. In P. Glimcher, & E. Fehr (Hrsg.), Neuroeconomics. Decision making and the brain (2. Aufl. S. 173–192). Amsterdam: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kable, J. W., & Glimcher, P. W. (2007). The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 10(12), 1625–1633.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, R. (Hrsg.). (2011). The Oxford Handbook of Free Will (2. Aufl.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karbach, J., & Unger, K. (2014). Executive control training from middle childhood to adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 390.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: a meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25(11), 2027–2037.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kazén, M., Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Self-infiltration vs. self-compatibility checking in dealing with unattractive tasks: The moderating influence of state vs. action orientation. Motivation and Emotion, 27(3), 157–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kazén, M., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2008). Individual differences in intention initiation under demanding conditions: Interactive effects of state vs. action orientation and enactment difficulty. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(3), 693–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald III, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303(5660), 1023–1026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kiefer, M. (2007). Top-down modulation of unconscious ‘automatic’ processes: A gating framework. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1–2), 289–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching – A review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Cilles, S. E., & Gold, B. T. (2011). Common and distinct mechanisms of cognitive flexibility in prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(13), 4771–4779.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2008). Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 317–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kober, H., Kross, E. F., Mischel, W., Hart, C. L., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). Regulation of craving by cognitive strategies in cigarette smokers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 106(1), 52–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. M. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: a review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(1), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koechlin, E., & Summerfield, C. (2007). An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(6), 229–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S., & Grafman, J. (1999). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature, 399(6732), 148–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koechlin, E., Ody, C., & Kounelher, F. (2003). The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex. Science, 302(5648), 1181–1185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision Making and the Avoidance of Cognitive Demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 139(4), 665–682.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koole, S. L., & Jostmann, N. B. (2004). Getting a Grip on Your Feelings: Effects of Action Orientation and External Demands on Intuitive Affect Regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), 974–990.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koole, S. L., & Kuhl, J. (2008). Dealing with unwanted feelings: The role of affect regulation in volitional action control. In J. Y. Shah, & W. L. Gardner (Hrsg.), Handbook of motivation science (S. 295–307). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotabe, H. P., & Hofmann, W. (2015). On integrating the components of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 618–638.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kräplin, A., Buehringer, G., Oosterlaan, J., Brink, W. van den, Goschke, T., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2014a). Dimensions and disorder specificity of impulsivity in pathological gambling. Addictive Behaviors, 39(11), 1646–1651.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kräplin, A., Dshemuchadse, M., Behrendt, S., Scherbaum, S., Goschke, T., & Buehringer, G. (2014b). Dysfunctional decision-making in pathological gambling: Pattern specificity and the role of impulsivity. Psychiatry Research, 215(3), 675–682.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kräplin, A., Behrendt, S., Scherbaum, S., Dshemuchadse, M., Bühringer, G., & Goschke, T. (2015). Increased impulsivity in pathological gambling: Considering nicotine dependence. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 37(4), 367–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krönke, K. M., Wolff, M., Benz, A., & Goschke, T. (2015). Successful smoking cessation is associated with prefrontal cortical function during a Stroop task: A preliminary study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 234(1), 52–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kubota, K. N. (1971). Prefrontal cortical unit activity and delayed alternation performance in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 34, 337–347.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating effect of state versus action orientation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 40(1), 155–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1982). Handlungskontrolle als metakognitiver Vermittler zwischen Intention und Handeln: Freizeitaktivitaten bei Hauptschulern. Zeitschrift fur Entwicklungspsychologie und Padagogische Psychologie, 14(2), 141–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1983). Motivation, Konflikt und Handlungskontrolle. Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional mediators of cognitive-behavior consistency: Self-regulatory processes and actions versus state orientation. In J. Kuhl, & J. Beckmann (Hrsg.), Action control: From cognition to behavior (S. 101–128). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1994). Action versus state orientation: Psychometric properties of the action-control scale (ACS-90). In J. Kuhl, & J. Beckmann (Hrsg.), Volition and personality: action versus state orientation (S. 47–59). Toronto/Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1996). Wille und Freiheitserleben. Formen der Selbststeuerung. In J. Kuhl, & H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Motivation, Volition und Handlung Enzyklopädie der Psychologie Serie IV, (Bd. 4, S. 665–765). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (2000). A functional-design approach to motivation and self-regulation: The dynamics of personality systems and interactions. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Hrsg.), Handbook of self-regulation (S. 111–169). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (2001). Motivation und Persönlichkeit. Interaktionen psychischer Systeme. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (2010). Individuelle Unterschiede in der Selbststeuerung. In J. Heckhausen, & H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Motivation und Handeln 4. Aufl. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1994). Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation. Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Goschke, T. (1994a). A theory of action control: Mental subsystems, modes of control, and volitional conflict-resolution strategies. In J. Kuhl, & J. Beckmann (Hrsg.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (S. 93–124). Göttingen/Toronto: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Goschke, T. (1994b). State orientation and the activation and retrieval of intentions in memory. In J. Kuhl, & J. Beckmann (Hrsg.), Volition and personality: action vs. state orientation (S. 127–153). Toronto/Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1994). Self-discrimination and memory: State orientation and false self-ascription of assigned activities. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 66(6), 1103–1115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Weiß, M. (1994). Performance deficits following uncontrollable failure: Impaired action control or global attributions and generalized expectancy deficits? In J. Kuhl, & J. Beckmann (Hrsg.), Volition and personality: action versus state orientation (S. 317–328). Toronto/Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. Cognition, 88(2), 223–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (2012). Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron, 73(4), 653–676.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflexive action control in patients with frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychology, 15(1), 80–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1926). Vorsatz, Wille und Bedürfnis. Psychologische Forschung, 7, 330–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lhermitte, F. (1983). ‘Utilization behavior’ and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106, 237–255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain, 3(106), 623–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linetzky, M., Pergamin-Hight, L., Pine, D. S., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2015). Quantitative evaluation of the clinical efficacy of attention bias modification treatment for anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 32(6), 383–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y., & Wang, Z. (2014). Positive affect and cognitive control: approach-motivation intensity influences the balance between cognitive flexibility and stability. Psychological Science, 25(5), 1116–1123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Motivational influences on cognitive control: Behavior, brain activation, and individual differences. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(1), 99–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G., Rick, S., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Neuroeconomics. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 647–672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1989). Automaticity and cognitive control. In J. S. Uleman, & J. A. Bargh (Hrsg.), Unintended thought (S. 52–74). New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. In D. Dagenbach, & T. H. Carr (Hrsg.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (S. 189–239). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, R. B., Hofmann, W., Wagner, D. D., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2014). Neural predictors of giving in to temptation in daily life. Psychological Science, 25(7), 1337–1344.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. London: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. Andover: Tavistock.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKillop, J., Amlung, M. T., Few, L. R., Ray, L. A., Sweet, L. H., & Munafo, M. R. (2011). Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 216(3), 305–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Clarke, P. J. F. (2015). The attentional bias modification approach to anxiety intervention. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(1), 58–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maier, S. U., Makwana, A. B., & Hare, T. A. (2015). Acute stress impairs self-control in goal-directed choice by altering multiple functional connections within the brain’s decision circuits. Neuron, 87(3), 621–631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., & Bink, M. L. (1998). Activation of completed, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 24(2), 350–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., & Bryan, E. S. (1999). The activation of unrelated and canceled intentions. Memory & Cognition, 27(2), 320–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maylor, E. A., Chater, N., & Brown, G. D. A. (2001). Scale invariance in the retrieval of retrospective and prospective memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(1), 162–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., & Keele, S. W. (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: The role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), 4–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., Awh, E., & Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience, 6(5), 450–452.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306(5695), 503–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meilan, J. J. G. (2008). Activation and deactivation processes in the postponed intention paradigm. Psychologia, 51(1), 89–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran, N., Chorev, Z., & Sapir, A. (2000). Component processes in task switching. Cognitive Psychology, 41(3), 211–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervag, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 483–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Trevena, J. A. (2002). Cortical movement preparation and conscious decisions: Averaging artifacts and timing biases. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 11(2), 308–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., Erickson, C. A., & Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(0), 5154–5167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card-sorting. Archives of Neurology, 9, 90–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Raskoff, Z. A. (1972). Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21(2), 204–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 687–696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Ayduk, O., Berman, M. G., Casey, B. J., Gotlib, I. H., Jonides, J., Kross, E., Teslovich, T., Wilson, N. L., Zayas, V., & Shoda, Y. (2010). ‘Willpower’ over the life span: decomposing self-regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(2), 252–256.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J.-C. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: The effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 115(2–3), 123–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moors, A. (2016). Automaticity: Componential, Causal, and Mechanistic Explanations. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 263–287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, S. E., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2012). Research Domain Criteria: cognitive systems, neural circuits, and dimensions of behavior. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14(1), 29–37.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nee, D. E., & Brown, J. W. (2012). Rostral-caudal gradients of abstraction revealed by multi-variate pattern analysis of working memory. Neuroimage, 63(3), 1285–1294.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1984). Automatic processing: a review of recent findings and a plea for an old theory. In W. Prinz, & A. F. Sanders (Hrsg.), Cognition and motor processes (S. 255–294). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct parameter specification? In C. Umilta, & M. Moscovitch (Hrsg.), Attention and performance 15: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (S. 123–150). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niendam, T. A., Laird, A. R., Ray, K. L., Dean, Y. M., Glahn, D. C., & Carter, C. S. (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(2), 241–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Hrsg.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research (Bd. 4, S. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nutt, D. J., Lingford-Hughes, A., Erritzoe, D., & Stokes, P. R. (2015). The dopamine theory of addiction: 40 years of highs and lows. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(5), 305–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Reilly, R. C. (2006). Biologically Based Computational Models of High-Level Cognition. Science, 314(5796), 91–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Reilly, R. C., & Frank, M. J. (2006). Making Working Memory Work: A Computational Model of Learning in the Prefrontal Cortex and Basal Ganglia. Neural Computation, 18(2), 283–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Reilly, R. C., Herd, S. A., & Pauli, W. M. (2010). Computational models of cognitive control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 257–261.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • O’Reilly, R. C., Hazy, T. E., & Herd, S. A. (im Druck). The Leabra cognitive architecture. In S. Chipman (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive science (Vol. 1). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215–1229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, A. M., Downes, J. J., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey, C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1990). Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 28, 1021–1034.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parks-Stamm, E. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Action control by implementation intentions: Effective cue detection and efficient response initiation. Social Cognition, 25(2), 248–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, M. (2006). Illusion Freiheit? Mögliche und unmögliche Konsequenzen der Hirnforschung (2. Aufl.). Frankfurt a.M.: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, M., & Roth, G. (2008). Freiheit, Schuld und Verantwortung. Grundzüge einer naturalistischen Theorie der Willensfreiheit. Frankfurt a.M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2010). Neural representations of subjective reward value. Behavioural Brain Research, 213(2), 135–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pezzulo, G., & Rigoli, F. (2011). The value of foresight: how prospection affects decision-making. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., & Goschke, T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3218–3227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plessow, F., Kiesel, A., & Kirschbaum, C. (2012a). The stressed prefrontal cortex and goal-directed behaviour: acute psychosocial stress impairs the flexible implementation of task goals. Experimental Brain Research, 216(3), 397–408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plessow, F., Schade, S., Kirschbaum, C., & Fischer, R. (2012b). Better not to deal with two tasks at the same time when stressed? Acute psychosocial stress reduces task shielding in dual-task performance. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(3), 557–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Hrsg.), Information processing and cognition (S. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (2004). Kritik des freien Willens: Bemerkungen über eine soziale Institution. Psychologische Rundschau, 55, 198–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W., Aschersleben, G., & Koch, I. (2013). Cognition and action. In E. Morsella, J. A. Bargh, & P. M. Gollwitzer (Hrsg.), Oxford handbook of human action (S. 35–71). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puca, R. M. S. (2001). The influence of the achievement motive on spontaneous thoughts in pre- and postdecisional action phases. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 302–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quirin, M., Bode, R. C., & Kuhl, J. (2011). Recovering from negative events by boosting implicit positive affect. Cognition & Emotion, 25(3), 559–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1984). Lapses of attention. In R. Parasuraman, & D. Davies (Hrsg.), Varieties of attention (S. 515–550). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redish, A. D. (2013). The Mind within the Brain: How we make decisions and how those decisions go wrong. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redish, A. D., Jensen, S., & Johnson, A. (2008). A unified framework for addiction: vulnerabilities in the decision process. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(4), 415–437.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, F. R., & Yeung, N. (2014). Neuroimaging studies of task switching. In J. A. Grange, & G. Houghton (Hrsg.), Task switching and cognitive control (S. 237–271). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Riggall, A. C., & Postle, B. R. (2012). The relationship between working memory storage and elevated activity as measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(38), 12990–12998.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rive, M. M., Rooijen, G. van, Veltman, D. J., Phillips, M. L., Schene, A. H., & Ruhe, H. G. (2013). Neural correlates of dysfunctional emotion regulation in major depressive disorder. A systematic review of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10 Pt 2), 2529–2553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, T. W., Gillan, C. M., Smith, D. G., Wit, S. de, & Ersche, K. D. (2012). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(1), 81–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2008). Review. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1507), 3137–3146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roca, M., Torralva, T., Gleichgerrcht, E., Woolgar, A., Thompson, R., Duncan, J., & Manes, F. (2011). The role of Area 10 (BA10) in human multitasking and in social cognition: a lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 49(13), 3525–3531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2014). Parallel Distributed Processing at 25: further explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cognitive Science, 38(6), 1024–1077.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E. T. (2010). Attractor networks. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1, 119–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rösler, F. (2005). Neuronale Korrelate der Handlungsausführung. Zur Validität der Experimente von Libet (1983). In D. Stederoth, & K. Köchy (Hrsg.), Willensfreiheit als interdisziplinäres Problem (S. 165–190). Freiburg i. Br: Alber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rösler, F. (2011). Psychophysiologie der Kognition – Eine Einführung in die kognitive Neurowissenschaft. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 104(1), 383–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruge, H., Jamadar, S., Zimmermann, U., & Karayanidis, F. (2013). The many faces of preparatory control in task switching: reviewing a decade of fMRI research. Human Brain Mapping, 34(1), 12–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruge, H., & Wolfensteller, U. (2010). Rapid Formation of Pragmatic Rule Representations in the Human Brain during Instruction-Based Learning. Cerebral Cortex, 20(7), 1656–1667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruge, H., & Wolfensteller, U. (2013). Functional integration processes underlying the instruction-based learning of novel goal-directed behaviors. Neuroimage, 68, 162–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruge, H., & Wolfensteller, U. (2015). Distinct fronto-striatal couplings reveal the double-faced nature of response-outcome relations in instruction-based learning. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(2), 349–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., Martin, V. C., Spreng, R. N., & Szpunar, K. K. (2012). The future of memory: remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron, 76(4), 677–694.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherbaum, S., Fischer, R., Dshemuchadse, M., & Goschke, T. (2011). The dynamics of cognitive control: evidence for within trial conflict adaptation from frequency tagged EEG. Psychophysiology, 48(5), 591–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherbaum, S., Dshemuchadse, M., Ruge, H., & Goschke, T. (2012). Dynamic goal states: adjusting cognitive control without conflict monitoring. Neuroimage, 63(1), 126–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, T. W., De Rosa, E., & Anderson, A. K. (2009). Opposing influences of affective state valence on visual cortical encoding. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(22), 7199–7207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schouwenburg, M. R. van, Aarts, E., & Cools, R. (2010). Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive control: Distinct roles for the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Current Pharmacological Design, 16(18), 2026–2032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. (2013). Updating dopamine reward signals. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(2), 229–238.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schurger, A., Sitt, J. D., & Dehaene, S. (2012). An accumulator model for spontaneous neural activity prior to self-initiated movement. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 109(42), E2904–2913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer, N. J., Saks, M. J., Murphy, E. R., Roskies, A. L., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & Gaudet, L. M. (2011). Neuroimages as evidence in a mens rea defense: no impact. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 17(3), 357–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Serences, J. T., Ester, E. F., Vogel, E. K., & Awh, E. (2009). Stimulus-specific delay activity in human primary visual cortex. Psychological Science, 20(2), 207–214.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(3), 154–167.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 298, 199–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727–741.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., Burgess, P. W., Schon, F., & Baxter, D. M. (1989). The origins of utilization behaviour. Brain, 112(Pt 6), 1587–1598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000). Using implementation intentions to increase attendance for cervical cancer screening. Health Psychology, 19, 283–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The Interplay Between Goal Intentions and Implementation Intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(1), 87–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217–240.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions towards the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinnot-Armstrong, W., & Nadel, L. (Hrsg.). (2011). Conscious will and responsibility. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H. J., & Haynes, J.-D. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11(5), 543–545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stelzel, C., Basten, U., & Fiebach, C. J. (2011). Functional connectivity separates switching operations in the posterior lateral frontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3529–3539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stiensmeier-Pelster, J. (1994). Choice of decision-making strategies and action versus state orientation. In J. Kuhl, & J. Beckmann (Hrsg.), Volition and personality: Action- versus state orientation (S. 167–176). Seattle: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strobach, T., Salminen, T., Karbach, J., & Schubert, T. (2014). Practice-related optimization and transfer of executive functions: a general review and a specific realization of their mechanisms in dual tasks. Psychological Research, 78(6), 836–851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (2013). Handbook of frontal lobe function. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szczepanski, S. M., & Knight, R. T. (2014). Insights into human behavior from lesions to the prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 83(5), 1002–1018.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y. Y., Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., & Volkow, N. D. (2015). Circuitry of self-control and its role in reducing addiction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(8), 439–444.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trevena, J. A., & Miller, J. (2010). Brain preparation before a voluntary action: Evidence against unconscious movement initiation. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 447–456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ullsperger, M., Bylsma, L. M., & Botvinick, M. (2005). The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(4), 467–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ullsperger, M., Danielmeier, C., & Jocham, G. (2014). Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior. Physiological Reviews, 94, 35–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: Interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 601–626.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vihvelin, K. (2013). Causes, laws, and free will. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Volkow, N. D., & Baler, R. D. (2015). NOW vs LATER brain circuits: implications for obesity and addiction. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(6), 345–352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vossel, S., Geng, J. J., & Fink, G. R. (2014). Dorsal and ventral attention systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist, 20(2), 150–159.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotski, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wallis, J. D., Anderson, K. C., & Miller, E. K. (2001). Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature, 411(6840), 953–956.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walser, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2012). The failure of deactivating intentions: Aftereffects of completed intentions in the repeated prospective memory cue paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 38(4), 1030–1044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walser, M., Goschke, T., & Fischer, R. (2014). The difficulty of letting go: moderators of the deactivation of completed intentions. Psychological Research, 78(4), 574–583.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walter, H., Kalckreuth, A. von, Schardt, D., Stephan, A., Goschke, T., & Erk, S. (2009). The temporal dynamics of voluntary emotion regulation. PLoS ONE, 4(8), e6726.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, D. A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46(4), 361–413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waszak, F., Cardoso-Leite, P., & Hughes, G. (2012). Action effect anticipation: Neurophysiological basis and functional consequences. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(2), 943–959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whelan, R., Conrod, P. J., Poline, J.-B., Lourdusamy, A., Banaschewski, T., Barker, G. J., & Garavan, H. (2012). Adolescent impulsivity phenotypes characterized by distinct brain networks. Nature Neuroscience, 15(6), 920–925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiers, R. W., Gladwin, T. E., Hofmann, W., Salemink, E., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2013). Cognitive bias modification and cognitive control training in addiction and related psychopathology: Mechanisms, clinical perspectives, and ways forward. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(2), 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (Hrsg.). (2009). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittchen, H.-U., Jacobi, F., Rehm, J., Gustavsson, A., Svensson, M., Jonsson, B., & Steinhausen, H. C. (2011). The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe 2010. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 21(9), 655–679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfensteller, U., & Ruge, H. (2012). Frontostriatal mechanisms in instruction-based learning as a hallmark of flexible goal-directed behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 192.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, G., & Allport, A. (2000). Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs”. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 212–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N., Nystrom, L. E., Aronson, J. A., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Between-task competition and cognitive control in task switching. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(5), 1429–1438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zanto, T. P., Rubens, M. T., Thangavel, A., & Gazzaley, A. (2011). Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 14(5), 656–661.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zeigarnik, B. (1927). Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Handlungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas Goschke .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Goschke, T. (2017). Volition und kognitive Kontrolle. In: Müsseler, J., Rieger, M. (eds) Allgemeine Psychologie. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-53898-8_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-53898-8_9

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-53897-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-53898-8

  • eBook Packages: Psychology (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics