Skip to main content

Sensory and Perceptual Control of Action in Early Human Development

  • Chapter
Relationships Between Perception and Action
  • 328 Accesses

Abstract

Although the idea is not new, it is only recently that we are frequently able to read that action and perception are two inseparable poles of human and animal behavior1; it is a theory (or dogma?) which is so attractive that it unifies the adherents of apparently incompatible schools of thought, such as the cognitivists and the followers of “ecological psychology.” Perception and action are inseparable in the sense that perception is constantly at the service of action, controlling and guiding action. Conversely, action may also be at the service of perception, by conditioning and determining it, although less research has been dedicated to the study of such a relationship (see Bridgeman, this volume, for a discussion of the role of eye movements in visual perception). Furthermore, it is through action that an objects properties are discovered. More specifically, most authors (Fowler Turvey, 1982; Neisser, 1985; Mounoud, 1981) agree in the recognition that perception and action are intricately related in the sense that a unique language may be used to describe, and possibly explain, the functioning of the structures of both perception and action. For Neisser it is very likely that mental structures (schemata) that organize movements are represented internally in the same format as those responsible for perceptions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abravanel, E., Sigafoos, A.D. (1984). Exploring the presence of imitation during early infancy. Child Development, 55, 381–392.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Atkinson, J. (1983). How does infant vision change in the first three months of life? Paper presented at the first meeting of the Groningen Study Group on Developmental Neurology, Groningen.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Banks, M.S., Salapatek, P. (1983). Infant visual perception. In P.H. Müssen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 2 ). New Yoik: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Bower, T.G.R., Broughton, J.–M., Moore, M.K. (1970a). The coordination of visual and tactual input in infants. Perception and Psychophysics, 8, 51–53.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Bower, T.G.R., Broughton, J.M., Moore, M.K. (1970b). Demonstration of intention in the reaching behavior of neonate humans. Nature, 5272, 679–681.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Bronson, G. (1982). Structures, states and characteristics of the nervous system at birth. In P. Stratton (Ed.), Psychobiology of the human newborn (pp. 93–118 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Bushneil, E.W. (1982). Visual–tactual knowledge in 8–, 9 1/2–, and 11–month–old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 5, 63–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bushnell, E.W. (1985). The decline of visually guided reaching during infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 139–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Castner, B.M. (1932). The development of fine prehension in infancy. Genetic Psychology Mono–graphs, 12, 105–193.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Dodwell, P.C., Muir, D.W., DiFranco, D. (1976). Responses of infants to visually presented objects. Science, 194, 209–211.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Dunkeid, J. (1978). The function of imitation in infancy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Edinbourgh.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Fantz, R., Fagan, J.F., Miranda, S.B. (1975). Early visual selectivity. In L.B. Cohen P. Salapatek (Eds.), Infant perception: from sensation to cognition (Vol. 1 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Field, J. (1976). Relation of young infants reaching behavior to stimulus distance and solidity. Developmental Psychology, 5, 444–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Field, J. (1977). Coordination of vision and prehension in young infants. Child Development, 48, 97– 103.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Field, T.M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Fontaine, R. (1982). Conditions dévocation des conduites imitatives chez lenfant de 0 à 6 mois. Un–published doctoral dissertation, University of Paris.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Fontaine, R. (1984). Imitative skill between birth and six months. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 323–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Forssberg, H. (1985). Ontogeny of human locomotor control: I. Infant stepping, supported locomotion and transition to independent locomotion. Experimental Brain Research, 57, 480–493.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Fowler, C.A., Turvey, M.–T. (1982). Observational perspective and descriptive level in perceiving and acting. In W.B. Weimer D.S. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic processes (Vol. 2, pp. 1–19 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Guernsey, M. (1982). Eine genetische Studie über Nachahnung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 107, 105– 178.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Halverson, H.M. (1931). An experimental study of prehension in infants by means of systematic cinema records. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 12, 107–285.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Hayes, L.A., Watson, J.S. (1981). Neonatal imitation: fact or artifact? Developmental Psychology, 17, 655–660.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Jacobson, S.W. (1979). Matching behavior in the young infant. Child Development, 50, 425–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Jeannerod, M. (1982). A two–step model for visuo–motor development. In T.G. Bever (Ed.), Regres–sions in mental development: basic phenomena and theories. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Jeannerod, M., Biguer, B. (1983). Visuo–motor mechanisms in reaching within extrapersonal space. In D.J. Ingle, R.J.W. Mansfield, M.A. Goodale (Eds.), Advances in the analysis of visual behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Kelso, S.J.A., Holt, K.G., Kugler, P.N., Turvey, M.T. (1980). On the concept of coordinate structures as dissipative structures: H. Empirical lines of convergence. In G.E. Stelmach J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior. Amsterdam: North–Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Kœpke, J.E., Hamm, M., Legerstee, M., Russell, M. (1983). Neonatal imitation: two failures to re–plicate. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 97–102.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Kugiumutzakis, J. (1985a). Imitation in newborns 10–45 minutes old. Uppsala Psychological Reports, 376, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Kugiumutzakis, J. (1985b). Development of imitation during the first six months of life. Uppsala Psychological Reports, 377, 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Kugler, P.N., Kelso, J.A.S., Turvey, M.–T. (1982). On the control and coordination of naturally developing systems. In J.A.S. Kelso J.E. Clarke (Eds.), The development of movement control and coordination. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Kuypers, H.G.S. (1962). Corticospinal connection: post–natal development in rhesus monkey. Science, 138, 678–680.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Lewis, M., Wolan–Sullivan, M. (1985). Imitation in the first six months of life. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 57, 315–333.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Lockman, J.J., Ashmead, D.H. (1983). Discontinuities in the development of manual behavior. In L.P. Iipsitt (Ed.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 2 ). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Lockman, J.J., Ashmead, D.H., Bushnell, E.W. (1984). The development of anticipatory hand orientation during infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 176–186.

    Google Scholar 

  35. MacKenzie, B., Over, B. (1983). Young infants fail to imitate facial and manual gestures. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 85–89.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Maratos, O. (1973). The origin and the development of imitation during the first six months of life. Un–published doctoral dissertation, University of Geneva.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Maratos, O. (1982). Trends in the development of imitation in early infancy. In T.G. Bever (Ed.), Re–gressions in mental development: basic phenomena and theories. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  38. McDonnell, P.M., Abraham, W.C. (1979). Adaptation to displacing prisms in human infants. Per–ception, 8, 175–185.

    Google Scholar 

  39. McGraw, M.B. (1940). Neuromuscular development of the human infant as exemplified in the achievement of erect locomotion. Journal of Pediatrics, 17, 747–777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Meltzoff, A.N., Moore, K.M. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Meltzoff, A.N., Moore, K.M. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Develop–ment, 31, 78–84.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Morrongiello, B., Rocca, P. (1986). Age–related changes in reaching behavior. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Los Angeles.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Mounoud, P. (1981). Cognitive development: construction of new structures or construction of internal organizations. In I.E. Sigel, D.M. Brodzinsky, R.M. Golinkoff (Eds.), New directions in piage– tian theory and practice. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Mounoud, P. (1983). Lévolution des conduites de préhension comme illustration dun modèle de développement. In S. de Schonen (Ed.), Les débuts du développement. Paris: PUF.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Mounoud, P., Bower, T.G.R. (1974). Conservation of weight in infants. Cognition, 3, 29–40.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Mounoud, P., Vinter, A. (1981). Representation and sensorimotor development. In G. Butterworth (Ed.), Infancy and epistemology. Brighton: Harvester.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Neisser, U. (1985). The role of invariant structures in the control of movement. In M. Frese J. Sabini (Eds.), Goal directed behavior: the concept of action in psychology (pp. 97–108 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Oppenheim, R.W. (1981). Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive processes in the development of the nervous system and behavior. In H.F.R. Prechtl K. Connolly (Eds.), Maturation and development. London: SIMP.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Paillard, J. (1971). Les déterminants moteurs de lorganisation de lespace. Cahiers de Psychologie, 4, 261–316.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Paillard, J. (1980). The multichannelling of visual cues and the organization of a visual guided re–sponse. In G.E. Stelmach J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior. Amsterdam: North–Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Paillard, J., Amblard, B. (1985). Static versus kinetic visual cues for the processing of spatial rela–tionships. In D.J. Ingle, M. Jeannerod, D.N. Lee (Eds.), Brain mechanisms and spatial vision (pp. 299–300 ). Dordrecht: Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Paillard, J., Beaubaton, D. (1976). Triggered and guided components of visual reaching. Their dissociation in split–brain studies. In M. Shabani (Ed.), The motor system: neurophysiology and muscle mechanism (pp. 333–347 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de lintelligence chez lenfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Piaget, J. (1946). La formation du symbole chez lenfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Prechtl, H.F.R. (1974). The behavioural states of the newborn infant. Brain Research, 76, 1304–1311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Preyer, W. (1887). Lame de lenfant, observations sur le développement psychique des premières années. Paris: Alcan.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Rader, N., Stem, J.D. (1980). Visually elicited reaching in neonates. Child Development, 53, 1004– 1007.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Ruff, H.A., Halton, A. (1978). Is there directed reaching in the human neonate? Developmental Psychology, 4, 425–426.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Animal Behavior, 27, 699–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Thelen, E. (1981). Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An ethological perspective. Developmental Psychology, 17, 231–251.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Thelen, E. ( 1984, March). Toward an action–based theory of infant development. Paper presented at the SRCD Study Group, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Thelen, E., Fisher, D.M. (1982). Newborn stepping: an explanation for a “disappearing” reflex. Developmental Psychology, 5, 760–775.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Thelen, E., Fisher, D.M. (1983). The organization of spontaneous leg movements in newborn infants. Journal of Motor Behavior, 4, 353–377.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Trevarthen, C. (1982). Basic patterns of psychogenetic change in infancy. In T.G. Bever (Ed.), Regressions in mental development: basic phenomena and theories. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Twitchell, T.E. (1965). The automatic grasping responses of infants. Neuropsychologia, 3, 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Twitchell, T.E. (1970). Reflex mechanisms and the development of prehension. In K. Connolly (Ed.), Mechanism of motor skill development. London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Valentine, C.W. (1930). The psychology of imitation with special reference to early childhood. Journal of Psychology, 2, 105–132.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Vinter, A. (1983). Imitation, représentation et mouvement. Doctoral dissertation, University of Geneva.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Vinter, A. (1985a). Limitation chez le nouveauné. Neuchâtel: Delachaux Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Vinter, A. (1985b). La capacité dimitation à la naissance: elle existe, mais que signifietelle? Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, 39, 16–33.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Vinter, A. (1986a). The role of movement in eliciting early imitations. Child Development, 57, 66–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Vinter, A. (1986b). A developmental perspective on behavioral determinants. Acta Psychologica, 63, 337–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Vinter, A. (1988). Perception of facial movements in early infancy: some reflections in relation to speech perception. In R. Campbell (Ed.), special issue of visible language, 23, 78–111.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Von Hoisten, C. (1979). Development of visually directed reaching: the approach phase. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 5, 160–178.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Von Hoisten, C. (1982). Eye-hand coordination in newborns. Developmental Psychology, 18, 450–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Von Hoisten, C. (1984). Developmental changes in the organization of pre–reaching movements. Developmental Psychology, 3, 378–388.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Von Hoisten, C., Fazel-Sandy, S. (1984). Developmental of visually guided orientation in reaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 208–219.

    Google Scholar 

  78. Wallon, H. (1942). De lacte à la pensée. Paris: Flammarion.

    Google Scholar 

  79. White, B.L., Castle, P., Held, R. (1964). Observations on the development of visually directed reaching. Child Development, 35, 349–364.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Yonas, A. (1979). Studies of spatial perception in infancy. In A.D. Pick (Ed.), Perception and its development: A tribute to Eleonor J. Gibson. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  81. Yonas, A., Cleaves, W.T., Petterson, L. (1978). Development of sensitivity to pictorial depth. Science, 200, 77–79.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Yonas, A., Granrud, C.E., Smith, I.M. (1982). Infants perceive accretion/deletion information for depth. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 22, 124.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Zanone, P.-G., Hauert, C.-A. (1987). I am a poor lonesome psychologist, far away from neurosciences. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 7, 109–130.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Zelazo, P.R., Zelazo, N., Kolb, S. (1972). “Walking” in the newborn. Science, 276,314–315.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1990 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Vinter, A. (1990). Sensory and Perceptual Control of Action in Early Human Development. In: Neumann, O., Prinz, W. (eds) Relationships Between Perception and Action. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-75348-0_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-75348-0_11

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-75350-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-75348-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics