Skip to main content

Semantically-Independent but Contextually-Dependent Interpretation of Contrastive Accent

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Prosodic Categories: Production, Perception and Comprehension

Part of the book series: Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory ((SNLT))

Abstract

A pair of eye-tracking experiments compared the effect of prominent pitch accent on pre-nominal intersective color adjectives and subsective size adjectives. Because subsective size adjectives are inherently contrastive, they may prompt comparison among a subset of referents regardless of the presence of prominent accent. In contrast, intersective color adjectives may require prosodic marking in order to be interpreted contrastively. Accent-driven contrast interpretation was tested within a real-world object manipulation paradigm, where participants followed pre-recorded instructions to decorate holiday trees. In both the Color and the Size experiments, a prominent accent (L + H*) on the adjective facilitated the detection of a contrastive target (e.g., Hang a red/medium star. → Next, hang a YELLOW/LARGE star.). When L + H* was infelicitously used in non-contrastive sequences (e.g., Hang a red/medium tree. → Next, hang a YELLOW/LARGE ball.), a reliable ‘garden-path’ increase in fixations to the incorrect contrastive competitor (e.g., yellow/large tree) was found in the Size but not in the Color experiment. As a result, the fixations to the correct target were visibly delayed in the Size experiment. In the Color experiment, the bias toward a contrastive interpretation was found regardless of accent type on the adjective. We argue that this was due to differences in the salience of visual contrast. While the present results confirm that L + H* on pre-nominal modifiers evokes an anticipatory contrast interpretation, they also suggest that the effect of accentual prominence is modulated by the discourse and referential context, rather than by the inherent semantics of accented words.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The window size was determined based on the distribution of fixation duration observed across the two experiments. The average fixation duration was 185 ms (SD = 123) for the Color and 192 ms (SD = 133) for the Size experiment. Across the two experiments, 90% of the fixations lasted less than 300 ms.

  2. 2.

    Due to the space limit, these mean function figures are not included here. These figures are available upon request to the first author.

  3. 3.

    The mean likelihood functions for the contrastive competitors showed the increase and the decrease within -300-to-600 ms window in both experiments.

References

  • Allopenna, Paul D., James, S. Magnuson, and Michael K. Tanenhaus. 1988. Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language 38: 419–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altmann, Gerry T. M. and Yuki Kamide. 2004. Now you see it, now you don’t: Mediating the Mapping between Language and the Visual World. In John M. Henderson and Fernanda Ferreira (eds.) The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action, 347–383. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, Dale J. 2008. Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language 59: 457–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, Dale J. and Austin F. Frank. 2009. Analyzing multinomial and time-series data. Workshop on Ordinary and Multilevel Modeling at 2009 CUNY Conference on Sentence Processing, UC Davis. March, 25, 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, Christine and John Kingston. 1994. Salient pitch cues in the perception of contrastive focus, In Paul Bosch and Rob van der Sandt (eds.) Focus and natural language processing. IBM Working Papers on Logic and Linguistics, v. 6, 1–10. Heidelberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckman, Mary E. and Gayle M. Ayers. 1997. Guidelines for ToBI labelling, vers 3.0 [manuscript]: Ohio State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckman, Mary E., Julia Hirschberg and Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel. 2005. The Original ToBI System and the Evolution of the ToBI Framework. In Sun-Ah Jun (ed.) Prosodic Typology: The Phonology of Intonation and Phrasing, 9–54. New York: Oxford University Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, Dwight L. 1961. Contrastive accent and contrastive stress. Language 37: 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brugos, Alejna, Nanette Veilleux, Mara Breen and Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel. 2008. The Alternatives (Alt) Tier for ToBI: Advantages of Capturing Prosodic Ambiguity. Poster presented at 4th Conference on Speech Prosody 2008, May, Brazil. http://aune.lpl.univ-aix.fr/∼sprosig/sp2008/papers/id072.pdf

  • Chierchia, Gennaro and Sally McConnell-Ginet. 2000. Meaning and Grammar: An Introduction to Semantics. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, Gennaro and Raymond Turner. 1988. Semantics and Property Theory. Linguistics and Philosophy 11: 261–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahan, Delphine, James S. Magnuson, and Michael K. Tanenhaus. 2001a. Time course frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: evidence from eye-movements. Cognitive Psychology 42: 317–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahan, Delphine, James S. Magnuson, Michael K. Tanenhaus and Ellen M. Hogan. 2001b. Subcategorical mismatches and the time course of lexical access: evidence for lexical competition. Language and Cognitive Processes 16: 507–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahan, Delphine, Michael K. Tanenhaus and Craig G. Chambers. 2002. Accent and reference resolution in spoken-language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 47: 292–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, John M. and Fernanda Ferreira. 2004. Scene Perception for Psycholinguists. In John M. Henderson and Fernanda Ferreira (eds.) The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action, 1–58. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Kiwako, Nobuyuki Jincho, Utako Minai, Naoto Yamane, and Reiko Mazuka. (ms. under revision). Intonation facilitates contrast resolution: Evidence from Japanese adults & 6-year olds.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Kiwako and Shari R. Speer. 2008. Anticipatory effect of intonation: Eye movements during instructed visual search. Journal of Memory and Language 58: 541–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Kiwako, Nobuyuki Jincho, Naoto Yamane, Utako Minai and Reiko Mazuka. 2009. Use of emphatic pitch prominence for contrast resolution: An eye-tracking study with 6-year old and adult Japanese listeners. Paper presented at Boston University Conference on Language Development 34, Boston, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, T. Florian. 2008. Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59: 434–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, Keith. 2008. Quantitative Methods in Linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamp, Johan. A. W. 1975. Two Theories about Adjectives. In Edward Keenan (ed.) Formal Semantics of Natural Language, 123 – 155. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamp, Hans and Barbara Partee. 1995. Prototype theory and compositionality. Cognition 57: 129–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladd, D. Robert and Rachel Morton. 1997. The perception of intonational emphasis: continuous or categorical? Journal of Phonetics 25: 313–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladd, D. Robert and Astrid Schepman. 2003. Sagging transitions’ between high pitch accents in English: experimental evidence. Journal of Phonetics 31: 81–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metusalem, Ross and Kiwako Ito. 2008. The role of L+H* pitch accent in discourse construction. Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008, an international conference, Campinas, Brazil. May. http://aune.lpl.univ-aix.fr/∼sprosig/sp2008/papers/id142.pdf

  • Olsson, Henrik. and Leo Poom,. 2005. Visual memory needs categories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 102 (24): 8776–8780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oltean, Stefan. 2007. On the Semantics of Adjectives. Studia Universitatis Babes-Bolyai – Pholologia: 155–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, Janet and Julia Hirschberg. 1990. The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In Phillip Cohen, Jerry Morgan and Martha E. Pollack (eds.) Intentions in Communication, 342–365. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedivy, Julie, Michael K. Tanenhaus, Craig G. Chambers, and Greg Carlson. 1999. Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation Cognition 71: 109–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, Anne. 1998. Feature binding, attention and object perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B 353: 1295–1306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, Anne. 2006. Object tokens, binding and visual memory. In Handbook of Binding and Memory: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience, eds. Hubert Zimmer, Axel Mecklinger, and U.lman Lindenberger, 315–338. New York, Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, Duane G., Michael K. Tanenhaus and Christine A. Gunlogson. 2008. Interpreting pitch accents in on-line comprehension: H* vs. L+H*. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal 32, 7: 1232–1244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, Andrea, Bettina Braun, Mathew W. Crocker. 2006. Finding Referents in Time: Eye-Tracking Evidence for the Role of Contrastive Accents. Language and Speech 49 (3): 367–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, Mary E. and Anne M. Treisman 2002. Binding in Short-Term Visual Memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 131 (1): 48–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The present research was supported by NIH grant DC007090. We thank Ping Bai for assistance with data analysis, and Laurie Maynell, Ross Metusalem, and Julie McGory for assistance with creating and ToBI-annotating our spoken stimuli, and the anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kiwako Ito .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ito, K., Speer, S.R. (2011). Semantically-Independent but Contextually-Dependent Interpretation of Contrastive Accent. In: Frota, S., Elordieta, G., Prieto, P. (eds) Prosodic Categories: Production, Perception and Comprehension. Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-0137-3_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics